2014
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00398
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Is there a bilingual advantage in the ANT task? Evidence from children

Abstract: Bilinguals have been shown to outperform monolinguals in a variety of tasks that do not tap into linguistic processes. The origin of this bilingual advantage has been questioned in recent years. While some authors argue that the reason behind this apparent advantage is bilinguals' enhanced executive functioning, inhibitory skills and/or monitoring abilities, other authors suggest that the locus of these differences between bilinguals and monolinguals may lie in uncontrolled factors or incorrectly matched sampl… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2

Citation Types

20
202
0
3

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 186 publications
(236 citation statements)
references
References 46 publications
20
202
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Hence, BFs allow for evaluating the strength of evidence not only for the presence of an association, but explicitly also for the absence of a proposed association. Accordingly, using BFs has become increasingly popular in the area of cognitive enhancement (e.g., Antón et al 2014;Clark et al 2017;De Simoni and von Bastian 2017;Guye and von Bastian 2017;Kirk et al 2014;Paap et al 2014;Sprenger et al 2013;von Bastian et al 2017;von Bastian and Oberauer 2013).Based on previous findings, we expected positive associations of motivation (Brose et al 2012), a growth mindset (Jaeggi et al 2014), and conscientiousness (Studer-Luethi et al 2012) with change in training performance. Regarding neuroticism, our expectations were less specific, given that previous literature reported evidence for a negative association of neuroticism with mean training performance and transfer effects, but not with training gains (e.g., Studer-Luethi et al 2012, 2016.…”
mentioning
confidence: 76%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Hence, BFs allow for evaluating the strength of evidence not only for the presence of an association, but explicitly also for the absence of a proposed association. Accordingly, using BFs has become increasingly popular in the area of cognitive enhancement (e.g., Antón et al 2014;Clark et al 2017;De Simoni and von Bastian 2017;Guye and von Bastian 2017;Kirk et al 2014;Paap et al 2014;Sprenger et al 2013;von Bastian et al 2017;von Bastian and Oberauer 2013).Based on previous findings, we expected positive associations of motivation (Brose et al 2012), a growth mindset (Jaeggi et al 2014), and conscientiousness (Studer-Luethi et al 2012) with change in training performance. Regarding neuroticism, our expectations were less specific, given that previous literature reported evidence for a negative association of neuroticism with mean training performance and transfer effects, but not with training gains (e.g., Studer-Luethi et al 2012, 2016.…”
mentioning
confidence: 76%
“…Hence, BFs allow for evaluating the strength of evidence not only for the presence of an association, but explicitly also for the absence of a proposed association. Accordingly, using BFs has become increasingly popular in the area of cognitive enhancement (e.g., Antón et al 2014;Clark et al 2017;De Simoni and von Bastian 2017;Guye and von Bastian 2017;Kirk et al 2014;Paap et al 2014;Sprenger et al 2013;von Bastian et al 2017;von Bastian and Oberauer 2013).…”
Section: The Present Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This advantage, which is supported by many studies (Bialystok, 1999;Costa et al, 2008), has been communicated to the general public through significant media coverage (Bhattacharjee, 2012;Reville, 2014). Yet, several recent studies have failed to replicate this finding (Morton and Harper, 2007;Paap and Greenberg, 2013;Antón et al, 2014), leading many researchers to doubt its validity (de Bruin et al, 2015;Paap et al, 2016), and creating a division in the bilingualism research community between believers and skeptics (Bak, 2016;Bialystok, 2016;Titone et al, 2017). This ambiguous state of the literature is not limited to executive functioning.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…Departing from previous studies that have utilised a single task to assess executive processes (c.f., Antón, et al, 2014;Bialystok, Craik, Grady, Chau, Ishii, Gunji, & Pantev, 2005;Bialystok & Viswanathan, 2009;Duñabeitia et al, 2014;Kirk, Fiala, Scott-Brown, & Kempe, 2014;Prior & Gollan, 2011) we were keen to employ multiple tasks measuring various aspects of attention and working memory to help identify more precisely which aspects of cognition are privileged in bilingualism. Previous studies have differentiated two processes related to attentionone that facilitates suppression of irrelevant information and the other that aids inhibiting prepotent response tendencies (e.g., Bunge, Dudukovic, Thomason, Vaidya & Gabrieli, 2002).…”
Section: Overview Of Present Studymentioning
confidence: 99%