2008
DOI: 10.1037/a0012628
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Is it better to be moral than smart? The effects of morality and competence norms on the decision to work at group status improvement.

Abstract: Three studies examined strategies of status improvement in experimentally created (Study 1 and 2) and pre-existing (Study 3) low-status groups. Theory and prior research suggested that an in-group norm that established a particular strategy of status improvement as moral (rather than competent) would have a greater effect on individuals' decision to work at this strategy. Both Study 1 and 2 found that morality norms had a greater impact than competence norms on individuals' decision to work at group (rather th… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

7
134
0
1

Year Published

2011
2011
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

4
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 121 publications
(142 citation statements)
references
References 62 publications
7
134
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…In a different way, people might be motivated to accurately verify the ingroup member's morality because the presence of an immoral affiliate within their own group is detrimental for the group as a whole. Indeed, an immoral member or someone disregarding the norms, is not functional to the goal's achievement, to the maximization of group benefits (De Waal, 1996;Leach et al, 2007), nor to the maintenance of a positive ingroup image (see Marques, Yzerbyt, & Leyens, 1988; see also Ellemers, Pagliaro, Barreto, & Leach, 2008). Future studies should explore these argumentations considering a wide range of intergroup contexts.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In a different way, people might be motivated to accurately verify the ingroup member's morality because the presence of an immoral affiliate within their own group is detrimental for the group as a whole. Indeed, an immoral member or someone disregarding the norms, is not functional to the goal's achievement, to the maximization of group benefits (De Waal, 1996;Leach et al, 2007), nor to the maintenance of a positive ingroup image (see Marques, Yzerbyt, & Leyens, 1988; see also Ellemers, Pagliaro, Barreto, & Leach, 2008). Future studies should explore these argumentations considering a wide range of intergroup contexts.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…First, this is the case because -as argued above -competence differences are generally considered less problematic than differences in moral values. Second, people are quite aware of how others perceive their moral behavior, and are less likely to consider how others regard their competence (Wojciszke, 1994), regardless of their status in society (Ellemers et al, 2008). Indeed, ingroup respect based on superior competence had relatively little impact on the tendency to see the self as a good group member (Spears, Ellemers, & Doosje, 2005).…”
Section: Adherence To Moral Norms and Ingroup Respectmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Previous research in which low status vs. high status groups were compared established that group status does not affect the perceived importance of morality over competence (Ellemers et al, 2008;Leach et al, 2007).…”
Section: Overview Of the Present Researchmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The study was replicated in Malaysia with Malay and Chinese subjects (Hewstone & Ward, 1985). If the tendency of ingroup members to regard themselves as, on average, morally better than out-group members (Ellemers, Pagliaro, Barreto, & Leach, 2008;Leach, Ellemers, & Barreto, 2007;Levine & Campbell, 1972;although, cf., De Freitas & Cikara, 2018) extends to assessments of their true selves, the mismatch hypothesis appears to predict the patterning of intergroup explanatory preferences. (Note that the model does not assume all agents are represented as maximally or equally virtuous.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%