2017
DOI: 10.24193/jebp.2017.1.3
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Interpretation biases in the intergenerational transmission of worry: A path analysis

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

2
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For instance, girls compared to boys tended to generate more adaptive responses and better problem-solving solutions to ambiguous hypothetical scenarios. Finally, a more recent study investigated the various pathways of worry transmission from mothers to their offspring (9-17-year-olds) via interpretation biases (Pasarelu et al 2017). The study demonstrated that worry in youth was directly associated with interpretation biases towards ambiguous scenarios and maternal worry.…”
Section: Interpretation Biasmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For instance, girls compared to boys tended to generate more adaptive responses and better problem-solving solutions to ambiguous hypothetical scenarios. Finally, a more recent study investigated the various pathways of worry transmission from mothers to their offspring (9-17-year-olds) via interpretation biases (Pasarelu et al 2017). The study demonstrated that worry in youth was directly associated with interpretation biases towards ambiguous scenarios and maternal worry.…”
Section: Interpretation Biasmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The parent report and self-report of other well-validated questionnaires were used to assess children and adolescents’ psychopathology: the Penn State Worry Questionnaire, child- and parent-version (PSWQ-C/P; Chorpita et al, 1997), the Social Anxiety Scale for adolescents (SAS-A; La Greca & Lopez, 1998), the Social Phobia and Anxiety Inventory for children (SPAI-C; Beidel et al, 1995), the empirically-based syndromes scales of the Child Behavioral Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2004) and Youth Self-Report (YSR; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2004), the Children’s Automatic Thoughts Scale (CATS; Schniering & Rapee, 2002), and the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson et al, 1988). These instruments were previously used with Romanian samples (Păsărelu & Dobrean, 2015; Păsărelu et al, 2015; Păsărelu, Dobrean, Balazsi, Podina, et al, 2017; Robe et al, 2021). See Supplementary Material for an extensive description of each scale (https://data.mendeley.com/datasets/n8gpdjzh2t/1).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This may be explained by the mixed findings both in earlier studies and in the more recent evidence (summarized in Subar & Rozenman, 2021 ). Some recent studies provided support for this link (Păsărelu et al, 2017 ; Podina et al, 2013 ) whereas others did not (Neil et al, 2019 ; Ooi et al, 2015 ). Moreover, the meta-analytic evidence for similarities in threat interpretations of parents and children must be cautiously evaluated in light of several limitations, including the relatively small number of available empirical studies, which precludes firm conclusions on this link.…”
Section: Do Parents and Children Share A Common Information Processin...mentioning
confidence: 98%