2013
DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.05.041
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Interplay of Hippocampus and Prefrontal Cortex in Memory

Abstract: Recent studies on the hippocampus and the prefrontal cortex have considerably advanced our understanding of the distinct roles of these brain areas in the encoding and retrieval of memories, and of how they interact in the prolonged process by which new memories are consolidated into our permanent storehouse of knowledge. These studies have led to a new model of how the hippocampus forms and replays memories and how the prefrontal cortex engages representations of the meaningful contexts in which related memor… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

68
899
4

Year Published

2014
2014
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 1,090 publications
(973 citation statements)
references
References 80 publications
68
899
4
Order By: Relevance
“…Data, however, suggest that these two memory systems interact in either a synergistic or competitive manner (Gheysen et al, 2011;Rose et al, 2011;Sadeh et al, 2011;Schott et al, 2006;Seger et al, 2011), depending on task demands (Delgado and Dickerson, 2012;Packard and Goodman, 2012;Poldrack and Foerde, 2008). Specifically, the declarative memory system involves the MTL cooperating with the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (Preston and Eichenbaum, 2013), and enables instance learning, the extraction and application of underlying associative or categorical rules (generalization) to new instances, as well as retrieval of ensuing episodic and semantic memories (Hoscheidt et al, 2010). The second non-declarative learning system allows for rapid, feedback-based learning of patterns predicting reward, via the recruitment of the basal ganglia (Delgado and Dickerson, 2012;Poldrack and Foerde, 2008), and is essential for habit learning (Liljeholm and O'Doherty, 2012;Schwabe and Wolf, 2013;Smith and Graybiel, 2013).…”
Section: Declarative Memorymentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…Data, however, suggest that these two memory systems interact in either a synergistic or competitive manner (Gheysen et al, 2011;Rose et al, 2011;Sadeh et al, 2011;Schott et al, 2006;Seger et al, 2011), depending on task demands (Delgado and Dickerson, 2012;Packard and Goodman, 2012;Poldrack and Foerde, 2008). Specifically, the declarative memory system involves the MTL cooperating with the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (Preston and Eichenbaum, 2013), and enables instance learning, the extraction and application of underlying associative or categorical rules (generalization) to new instances, as well as retrieval of ensuing episodic and semantic memories (Hoscheidt et al, 2010). The second non-declarative learning system allows for rapid, feedback-based learning of patterns predicting reward, via the recruitment of the basal ganglia (Delgado and Dickerson, 2012;Poldrack and Foerde, 2008), and is essential for habit learning (Liljeholm and O'Doherty, 2012;Schwabe and Wolf, 2013;Smith and Graybiel, 2013).…”
Section: Declarative Memorymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The role of sleep in memory consolidation as well as generalization is well established (Rasch and Born, 2013;Stickgold and Walker, 2013). In reward memory formation, dopamine, in addition to enabling late-LTP, contributes to offline consolidation processes that continue after a stimulus-stimulus or stimulus-response learning and help better remember the learned association (Shohamy and Adcock, 2010;Wimber et al, 2011).…”
Section: Box 2: Role Of Sleep In the Consolidation And Generalizationmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…when experiencing a familiar environment in the dark. This operation follows the idea of a schema [20] [39]. Schemas can be thought of as generative models for a particular basic system [43].…”
Section: Characteristics Of Biological Autobiographical Memorymentioning
confidence: 99%