1996
DOI: 10.1121/1.415867
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Internal structure of phonetic categories: Evidence for within-category trading relations

Abstract: Phonetically relevant acoustic properties perceptually trade against each other at phonetic category boundaries. The present investigation used a category goodness judgment task to examine whether such properties also trade against each other in specifying the best exemplars of phonetic categories. The experiments focused on the say–stay distinction, specified by F1 onset frequency and silence duration preceding F1 onset. The main experiment demonstrated a within-category trading relation, such that as F1 onse… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
17
0

Year Published

1999
1999
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 20 publications
(19 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
2
17
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This difference in height between the two functions may have occurred because of a tendency for the subjects to rate stimuli more favorably when in a real-word context than when in a nonword context (peace vs. peef ). We have also found differences in the relative height of functions in some of our previous studies involving acousticphonetic contextual variables (Hodgson & Miller, 1996;Volaitis & Miller, 1992;Wayland et al, 1994), but in each case, as in the present study, the differences in height were not critical to the purpose of the investigation. As was outlined above, our question concerns the locations of the best-exemplar ranges and boundary indices along the VOT continuum.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 48%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…This difference in height between the two functions may have occurred because of a tendency for the subjects to rate stimuli more favorably when in a real-word context than when in a nonword context (peace vs. peef ). We have also found differences in the relative height of functions in some of our previous studies involving acousticphonetic contextual variables (Hodgson & Miller, 1996;Volaitis & Miller, 1992;Wayland et al, 1994), but in each case, as in the present study, the differences in height were not critical to the purpose of the investigation. As was outlined above, our question concerns the locations of the best-exemplar ranges and boundary indices along the VOT continuum.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 48%
“…Thus, the best exemplars of the perceptual category track the acousticphonetic variation associated with speaking rate in speech production. Similar parallels between speech production and the location of the best exemplars of the corresponding perceptual category have also been found for other acoustic-phonetic contextual factors so far examined (Volaitis & Miller, 1992;Wayland et al, 1994; and see Hodgson & Miller, 1996, for a similar productionbased best-exemplar shift due to within-category trading relations). These findings are consistent with the view that perceptual shifts in the location of best exemplars arise from a perceptual mechanism that operates so as to track acoustic-phonetic variation in production.…”
mentioning
confidence: 54%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Each of these cues occurs at a different time in the speech stream. When listeners recognize speech, the interpretation of one feature cue appears to be modulated by the value of other cues to that feature (Best, Morrongiello, & Robson, 1981;Hodgson & Miller, 1996;Parker, Diehl, & Kluender, 1986;Repp, 1982;Sinnott & Saporita, 2000;Summerfield & Haggard, 1977;Treisman, 1999). The existence of such trading relationships suggests that the listener integrates temporally dispersed cues to the same feature.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%