2019
DOI: 10.1007/s13164-019-00452-x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Intention at the Interface

Abstract: I identify and characterize the kind of personal-level control-structure that is most relevant for skilled action control, namely, what I call, "practical intention". I differentiate between practical intentions and general intentions not in terms of their function or timing but in terms of their content. I also highlight a distinction between practical intentions and other control mechanisms that are required to explain skilled action. I'll maintain that all intentions, general and practical, have the functio… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
9
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
0
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…First, the similarity between student u and other students and the similarity between student u's employment units and other employment units are calculated, and the employment units of the most dissimilar top N students and the most dissimilar top N units are selected as the high confidence negative sample set. en, the negative samples are randomly sampled from the negative sample set to generate negative samples for training [28,29].…”
Section: Establishment Of Recommendation Systemmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…First, the similarity between student u and other students and the similarity between student u's employment units and other employment units are calculated, and the employment units of the most dissimilar top N students and the most dissimilar top N units are selected as the high confidence negative sample set. en, the negative samples are randomly sampled from the negative sample set to generate negative samples for training [28,29].…”
Section: Establishment Of Recommendation Systemmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As I've argued elsewhere, we can distinguish between general and practical intentions, where general intentions specify the general goal or end of the task and practical (instrumental) intentions fill in the means or way of achieving that goal (Fridland 2019b). When it comes to skill, the most relevant kind of intentions are the practical ones, since differences at the level of practical intention are likely to reveal differences in skill level whereas both skilled and unskilled agents could easily share general intentions.…”
Section: Strategic Control and Practical Intentionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The role of motor schemas I will adopt a common model according to which actions are controlled by way of hierarchically arranged goal representations and processes (see Fridland, 2019;Mylopoulos & Pacherie, 2017Pacherie, 2008). The levels of the hierarchy are causally structured in a means-end way, such that the representations embedded within a given level specify the means of implementing the goal representations, if any, on the level(s) above them.…”
Section: Intention Implementationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This is a proximal intention. Proximal intentions can be understood as representations that (a) specify an agent's goal or plan, (b) unlike distal intentions are presentdirected and tokened in the situation of action, and (c) function to help structure and guide an action's course to completion (see e.g., Fridland, 2019;Pacherie, 2008, for further discussion of the functional role of proximal intention). I will say more about how such guidance works later on, but for now, the point is merely that it is this kind of intention that is at issue when we talk of action slips as involving a failure to do what one intends to do: When I slip, I fail to do what I proximally intend to do.…”
Section: The General Character Of Action Slipsmentioning
confidence: 99%