1985
DOI: 10.1080/10862968509547533
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Instructional Objectives in Text: Managing the Reader's Attention

Abstract: It is well established that the provision of instructional objectives before reading a text increases the learning of objective-relevant material in the text. The purpose of the present study was to identify some of the mechanisms by which objectives affect learning. College students studied text under three conditions: with specific objectives, with a general objective, and with no objectives. The objective-relevant material was located either high or low in the content structure of the text. The dependent me… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
4
0

Year Published

1988
1988
2014
2014

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 15 publications
1
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We have replicated Britton et al's (1985) previous findings and those of others by demonstrating that the provision of instructional objectives can enhance students' recall of information regardless of its height in the content structure. Of greater importance, we have shown that this way of cuing students' selective attention actually promotes real-time rehearsal activity—that is, the activity of mentally repeating, paraphrasing, and summarizing content so as to commit it to memory.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 85%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…We have replicated Britton et al's (1985) previous findings and those of others by demonstrating that the provision of instructional objectives can enhance students' recall of information regardless of its height in the content structure. Of greater importance, we have shown that this way of cuing students' selective attention actually promotes real-time rehearsal activity—that is, the activity of mentally repeating, paraphrasing, and summarizing content so as to commit it to memory.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 85%
“…If we can find out how objectives increase learning, researchers and authors may be able to increase the effectiveness of the objectives that they design. In the first study, Britton, Glynn, Muth, and Penland (1985) replicated the finding that objectives increase the recall of objective-relevant text content. Of more importance, however, was that they demonstrated that objectives also increase the amount of time that students spend reading the objective relevant content, as well as the mental effort that students use.…”
mentioning
confidence: 63%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Prior free recall may lead to better organization and increased accessibility of details of the witnessed event, which may in turn facilitate comprehension and encoding of the postevent narrative (Arnold & McDermott, 2013;Wissman, Rawson, & Pyc, 2011). It is also possible that by completing the initial free recall test, participants are better able to anticipate the level of details required to perform well on any subsequent memory tests, thus causing a shift in encoding strategy towards remembering details (Britton, Glynn, Muth, & Penland, 1985;McCrudden & Schraw, 2007). Finally, performing an initial free recall test provides participants with information about what they have learned well and what they have not (Lachman & Laughery, 1968;Padilla-Walker & Poole, 2002;Thompson, Wenger, and Bartling, 1978).…”
Section: Cognitive Mechanisms Underlying Retrieval-enhanced Suggestibmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A computer search (ERIC and PSY-CHINFO) from 1975 (since the Barnes and Clawson review) through 1990 using the key words "advance organizer" identified over 100 studies for review. Included were a variety of prereading activities such as studentgenerated text headings (e.g., Brooks et al, 1983); passage or paragraph headings (e.g., Doctorow et aL, 1978;Hartley et al, 1980;Watson, 1981); previews (e.g., Graves et aL, 1983); graphic or pictorial preorganizers (e.g., Dean and Enemoh, 1983;Mayer, 1983;Patrick and Evans, 1983;Thelen, 1979); information mapping (e.g., Jonassen, 1981); advance, guiding, or student-generated questions (e.g., Doctorow et al, 1978;Fowler and Lamberg, 1979;Hansen, 1981;Herber and Nelson, 1975;Ramsel and Grabe, 1983;Rickards and Denner, 1979); study goals or objectives (e.g., Britton et al, 1985;Rothkopf and Koether, 1978); outlines (e.g., Darch and Gersten, 1986;Glynn et al, 1985;Glynn and DiVesta, 1977;Guthrie, 1978;Thomas and Cummings, 1978); descriptions of text organization (e.g., Barnett, 1984;Schumacher et al, 1975;Slater et al, 1985;Snouffer and Thistlethwait, 1980); summaries before reading (e.g., Cook, 1981;Hartley et al, 1979); and student predictions (e.g., Hansen, 1981). Although these activities may prove to be beneficial reading aids, they would be more satisfactorily classifted as a type of adjunct aid (i.e., response modes, objectives, or questions; see Faw and Waller, 1976 for a more thorough discussion) other than advance organizer.…”
Section: Criticism Of and Changes In The Theorymentioning
confidence: 99%