2015
DOI: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.05.012
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Individual differences in anticipatory activity to food rewards predict cue-induced appetitive 50-kHz calls in rats

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
24
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
2

Relationship

2
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 32 publications
(25 citation statements)
references
References 66 publications
0
24
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This USV response habituated in both groups over time, perhaps indicating a decreasing appetitive state separable from the approach behaviors observed throughout the session. Additionally, explanation for our low rate of cue-induced USVs from either group may be a result of several factors, including predictability of the cue presentations, (Burgdorf et al 2000; Burgdorf et al 2005), food deprivation (Brenes and Schwarting 2014; Knutson et al 2002), the nature of the CS (auditory vs visual) (Brenes and Schwarting 2015; Meyer et al 2014), or the form of the approach measured (Brenes and Schwarting 2015). Thus, whether or not a cue elicits the initiation and/or enhancement in USVs is dependent on the specifics of the paradigm, rather than being a consistent response to a broad class of reward-associated stimuli.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This USV response habituated in both groups over time, perhaps indicating a decreasing appetitive state separable from the approach behaviors observed throughout the session. Additionally, explanation for our low rate of cue-induced USVs from either group may be a result of several factors, including predictability of the cue presentations, (Burgdorf et al 2000; Burgdorf et al 2005), food deprivation (Brenes and Schwarting 2014; Knutson et al 2002), the nature of the CS (auditory vs visual) (Brenes and Schwarting 2015; Meyer et al 2014), or the form of the approach measured (Brenes and Schwarting 2015). Thus, whether or not a cue elicits the initiation and/or enhancement in USVs is dependent on the specifics of the paradigm, rather than being a consistent response to a broad class of reward-associated stimuli.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Considering that laboratory rodents prefer larger and complex cages, social contact, and exercise (Olsson and Dahlborn, 2002;Van Loo et al, 2003;Heyse et al, 2015), we assume that EE is rewarding for rodents. Rats emit high-frequency (e.g., 50-kHz) USVs in social (e.g., mating and rough-and-tumble play) and non-social rewarding situations (e.g., brain stimulation of reward centers and administration of psychostimulants) (Burgdorf et al, 2000;Williams and Undieh, 2010;Pereira et al, 2014;Brenes and Schwarting, 2015). Out of the different 50-kHz calls, the FM subtypes (e.g., step-flats, step-trills, and trills) are indicative of a high, positive affective state (Burgdorf et al, 2007;Burgdorf et al, 2008;Burgdorf et al, 2011).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For instance, the between-group differences in the number of 50-kHz calls were 2.28 and 3.67-fold higher in the REE group, with the lowest call rate of the REE rats being even higher than the highest point of the CEE rats. Considering that 50-kHz calls may signal a state of appetitive incentive motivation (Brenes and Schwarting, 2014;Brenes and Schwarting, 2015), the differences in USVs suggest that unpredictable and restricted access to the EE cage could have extended and enhanced the rewarding properties of the EE stimuli. In support to the latter, we have previously found that rats trained to run a runway to enter a locked running wheel, displayed almost the same latencies and rates of 50-kHz calls than those with the unlocked wheel, suggesting that the structural features of the wheels were equally attractive for both groups (Heyse et al, 2015).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Indeed, increased anticipatory motor behavior was positively correlated with frequency of high ultrasonic vocalizations (Brenes and Schwarting, 2015). High ultrasonic vocalizations were emitted in anticipation of several rewarding stimuli, including food (Buck et al , 2014b; Opiol et al , 2015), a cocaine or ethanol reward (Buck et al , 2014a; Ma et al , 2010), and being reunited with a cage mate after a period of social isolation (Willey and Spear, 2012).…”
Section: Translational Assessments Of Reward and Motivationmentioning
confidence: 99%