1976
DOI: 10.1016/s0022-1031(76)80006-6
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Illusory correlation in interpersonal perception: A cognitive basis of stereotypic judgments

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

22
584
4
24

Year Published

1996
1996
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 720 publications
(634 citation statements)
references
References 9 publications
22
584
4
24
Order By: Relevance
“…Explanations that focus on a single dimension of evaluation cannot explain how the two groups would each be associated with a different attribute. For example, the original distinctiveness-based account (Hamilton & Gifford, 1976) posits only that Group B will be seen as possessing the rare attribute more so than Group A. It cannot explain why Group A would be seen to possess the common trait to a greater extent than Group B. Obversely, Rothbart's (1981) account proposes only that Group A will be seen as possessing the common trait to a greater extent than Group B but not that Group B will possess the rare trait to a greater extent than Group A.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…Explanations that focus on a single dimension of evaluation cannot explain how the two groups would each be associated with a different attribute. For example, the original distinctiveness-based account (Hamilton & Gifford, 1976) posits only that Group B will be seen as possessing the rare attribute more so than Group A. It cannot explain why Group A would be seen to possess the common trait to a greater extent than Group B. Obversely, Rothbart's (1981) account proposes only that Group A will be seen as possessing the common trait to a greater extent than Group B but not that Group B will possess the rare trait to a greater extent than Group A.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, the effects are reversed when the majority of the behaviors are negative rather than positive. In this case, Group B is perceived to be more positive than Group A (Hamilton & Gifford, 1976). tive versus negative behaviors for Group A (18 Ϫ 8 ϭ 10) than for Group B (9 Ϫ 4 ϭ 5) constitutes a real group difference-that Group A is objectively more favorable than Group B. They further suggested that participants are motivated to differentiate the two groups and therefore seek to amplify this real group difference via category accentuation processes, such as biased perceptions of group behaviors (e.g., Berndsen, Van der Pligt, Spears, & McGarty, 1996) and other confirmatory hypothesis-testing strategies (e.g., Berndsen, McGarty, Van der Pligt, & Spears, 2001).…”
Section: Illusory Correlation: Stereotype Formation In the Absence Ofmentioning
confidence: 97%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Research using both the minimal group paradigm (Tajfel, 1970;Tajfel, Billig, Bundy, & Flament, 1971) and the illusory correlation paradigm (Hamilton & Gifford, 1976) demonstrated differentiation between groups for which there was no informational basis. Participants in those studies were given information about groups that they used to make group judgments.…”
Section: Theoretical Context: Implications Of Stigs For Stereotype Fomentioning
confidence: 99%