2016
DOI: 10.1037/xlm0000189
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Identity priming consistently affects perceptual fluency but only affects metamemory when primes are obvious.

Abstract: Perceptual fluency manipulations influence metamemory judgments, with more fluently perceived information judged as more memorable. However, it is not always clear whether this influence is driven by actual experienced processing fluency or by beliefs about memory. The current study used an identity-priming paradigm-in which words are preceded by either matched (identical) or mismatched primes-to examine the 2 influences. Participants named and made judgments of learning (JOLs) for critical words and then comp… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
51
2

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 33 publications
(57 citation statements)
references
References 31 publications
1
51
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Experiment 4 provides more evidence for theory-based processes, as participants produced higher JOLs for truth than for lies, despite the lack of objective response latency differences. Third, participants can be directly asked about their beliefs through questionnaires without being exposed to the experimental materials (Besken, 2016;Koriat et al, 2004;Kornell et al, 2011;Mueller et al, 2014;Susser et al, 2016), make JOLs before they are exposed to the specific episode on an item-by-item basis (Mueller et al, 2014(Mueller et al, , 2016, or make global estimations about the predicted memory performance before the experiment begins (Frank & Kuhlmann, 2016). Experiment 5 showed the contribution of a priori beliefs to JOLs through the presentation of a scenario, in which participants had to predict their memory performance without exposure to the experiment, revealing higher memory predictions for truth trials than lie trials.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Experiment 4 provides more evidence for theory-based processes, as participants produced higher JOLs for truth than for lies, despite the lack of objective response latency differences. Third, participants can be directly asked about their beliefs through questionnaires without being exposed to the experimental materials (Besken, 2016;Koriat et al, 2004;Kornell et al, 2011;Mueller et al, 2014;Susser et al, 2016), make JOLs before they are exposed to the specific episode on an item-by-item basis (Mueller et al, 2014(Mueller et al, , 2016, or make global estimations about the predicted memory performance before the experiment begins (Frank & Kuhlmann, 2016). Experiment 5 showed the contribution of a priori beliefs to JOLs through the presentation of a scenario, in which participants had to predict their memory performance without exposure to the experiment, revealing higher memory predictions for truth trials than lie trials.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Perhaps, this finding can be explained by a priori beliefs about the effects of the lie-generation manipulation. Thus, Experiment 5 applied a straightforward, widely used method (Besken, 2016;Koriat et al, 2004;Kornell et al, 2011;Mueller et al, 2014;Susser et al, 2016) to measure the effects of a priori beliefs on memory predictions. Specifically, participants who have not been exposed to the study materials are asked to read a memory experiment scenario and make predictions about the effect of the lie-generation manipulation.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, in the present context in which valence accounted for the impact of emotion on JOLs, it seems reasonable that younger and older adults based their JOLs on beliefs about how emotion influences their memory (cf. Jia et al, 2016; Mueller, Dunlosky, Tauber, & Rhodes, 2014; Mueller, Tauber, & Dunlosky, 2013; Susser, Jin, & Mulligan, 2016) rather than on processing experiences (akin to fluency) during study (cf. Hertzog, Dunlosky, Robinson, & Kidder, 2003; Kelley & Jacoby, 1996; Undorf & Erdfelder, 2011, 2013).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Objective fluency is defined as some measure of processing speed, like reaction times. In contrast, subjective fluency, and thus the basis of subsequent behavior, may not only depend on the perceived ease of processing (as captured by objective measures), but also on additional factors, like the expected fluency, metacognitions, and attributional processes (see, e.g., Susser, Jin, & Mulligan, 2016, on metamnemonic beliefs). For example, participants may well differentiate whether experienced fluency stems from the retrieved object itself, or rather from some external source, the so-called context (previous encounters, perceptual conditions, etc.…”
Section: Small Impact Of Experimental Proceduresmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition to the metamnemonic and attributional processes discussed above (see also Susser et al, 2016), the process of recognition may be composed of several subprocesses, some or all of which contribute more or less to subjectively experienced fluency (Reber et al, 2004). Thus, there are several options for how to operationalize objective fluency: For example, Benjamin, Bjork, and Schwartz (1998) used answer retrieval latencies; Reber et al (2004), identification latencies; Schooler and Hertwig (2005), recognition latencies; Mueller, Dunlosky, Tauber, and Rhodes (2014), lexical decision latencies; Susser et al (2016), naming latencies; and Undorf and Erdfelder (2015), the numbers of trials to acquisition and self-paced study times (see also Koriat, 2008). This diversity of measures led Reber et al (2004, p. 50) to claim that "there seemingly is no single objective fluency."…”
Section: Assessment Of Fluencymentioning
confidence: 99%