1988
DOI: 10.5951/jresematheduc.19.3.0215
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Identifying Fractions on Number Lines

Abstract: This study investigated the ways students represented fractions on number lines and the effects of instruction on those representations. Two clinical teaching experiments and one large-group teaching experiment were conducted with fourth and fifth graders (N = 5, 8, and 30) The instruction primarily concerned representing fractions and ordering fractions on number lines. Tests and videotaped interviews indicated that unpartitioning, in particular, is difficult for students, although the instruction seemed to h… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

0
16
0
1

Year Published

2007
2007
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 34 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 3 publications
0
16
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Similarly, the difference between the part-whole and measurement interpretations could be since the part-whole interpretation is the first one child learns about fractions. On the other hand, another reason for this could be that, as Bright et al (1988) and Toluk (2002) pointed out, the constant use of the part-whole Sakarya University Journal of Education interpretation promotes the belief that fractions are part of a whole rather than referring to a number. It may also be because, as Contreras and Martinez-Cruz (2000), Park et al (2013) have highlighted, teachers who have been taught predominantly using the part-whole interpretation throughout their education do not spend much time in their classrooms for interpretations other than the part-whole interpretation of fractions.…”
Section: Discussion and Suggestionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Similarly, the difference between the part-whole and measurement interpretations could be since the part-whole interpretation is the first one child learns about fractions. On the other hand, another reason for this could be that, as Bright et al (1988) and Toluk (2002) pointed out, the constant use of the part-whole Sakarya University Journal of Education interpretation promotes the belief that fractions are part of a whole rather than referring to a number. It may also be because, as Contreras and Martinez-Cruz (2000), Park et al (2013) have highlighted, teachers who have been taught predominantly using the part-whole interpretation throughout their education do not spend much time in their classrooms for interpretations other than the part-whole interpretation of fractions.…”
Section: Discussion and Suggestionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For instance, the reason why the mean score of the regional area representation is greater than the other representations, in general, may be because it is the most used representation by teachers as it supports the part-whole interpretation when introducing fractions (Armstrong & Larson, 1995;Baykul, 2005;Hull, 2005). However, the fact that the number line representation has the lowest mean score in all research settings can be explained by the fact that the number line representation corresponds to the measurement interpretation, which is difficult to learn, that the number line representation has two separate references points when specifying fractions, and so on (Bright et al, 1988). Moreover, students have problems dividing the whole into equal parts and, accordingly, breaking down points between 0 and 1 either into more or fewer parts than the required parts, which can also be listed as the reasons why students have difficulty representing a number line (Aliustaoğlu, Tuna, & Biber, 2018;Pesen, 2008;Wong & Evans, 2011).…”
Section: Discussion and Suggestionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Research indicates that students have difficulty with using the number line to represent fractions (Baturo & Cooper, 1999;Behr et al, 1983;Bright, Behr, Post, & Wachsmuth, 1988;Larson, cited in Ni, 2000;Novilllis, cited in Ni, 2000). Hart (1989), in talking of the Concepts in Secondary Mathematics and Science (CSMS) study, indicated that scrutiny of all the easy CSMS items "suggested many secondary-aged children worked entirely within the set of whole numbers" (p. 46).…”
Section: Issues With Understanding the Measure Sub-construct Of Fractionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…or or Bright et al (1988) report three teaching experiments, including a follow up to the work on visual distractors. They identify that the number line is different from other fraction representations.…”
Section: Issues With Understanding the Measure Sub-construct Of Fractionsmentioning
confidence: 99%