2013
DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2013.01.007
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

How brooding minds inhibit negative material: An event-related fMRI study

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

0
29
0
1

Year Published

2013
2013
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 34 publications
(31 citation statements)
references
References 48 publications
(36 reference statements)
0
29
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Given brooding refers to a tendency to maintain attention to negative stimuli (Joormann et al, 2006), high-brooders would tend to experience greater difficulties in disengaging from negative information, possibly due to lower efficiency of DLPFC functioning. Thus, among high-brooders, the left DLPFC may be recruited to greater extents to compensate for this inefficiency (Vanderhasselt et al, 2011, 2013; Wang et al, 2015), possibly through upregulating positive affective processing, which then gradually lead to increase in DLPFC structural volume (Draganski et al, 2004; Scholz et al, 2009; Wang et al, 2015). Failure to engage in this compensatory process could lead to greater affective dysregulation and more negative affective states, coupled with reduced DLPFC volume as observed in major depressive patients (Grieve et al, 2013; Lai, 2013).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Given brooding refers to a tendency to maintain attention to negative stimuli (Joormann et al, 2006), high-brooders would tend to experience greater difficulties in disengaging from negative information, possibly due to lower efficiency of DLPFC functioning. Thus, among high-brooders, the left DLPFC may be recruited to greater extents to compensate for this inefficiency (Vanderhasselt et al, 2011, 2013; Wang et al, 2015), possibly through upregulating positive affective processing, which then gradually lead to increase in DLPFC structural volume (Draganski et al, 2004; Scholz et al, 2009; Wang et al, 2015). Failure to engage in this compensatory process could lead to greater affective dysregulation and more negative affective states, coupled with reduced DLPFC volume as observed in major depressive patients (Grieve et al, 2013; Lai, 2013).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Consistent with this, depressive patients show both DLPFC structural abnormalities and functional hypoactivity in resting-state and task-based fMRI (Gotlib and Hamilton, 2008; Koenigs and Grafman, 2009). Among healthy participants, in task-based fMRI, healthy subjects with high brooding tendency showed more DLPFC activations when trying to disengage from negative information compared to those with low brooding tendency (Vanderhasselt et al, 2011, 2013). Similarly, among major depressive patients, rumination induction elicited greater activations in the DLPFC than a distraction task (Cooney et al, 2010).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Nevertheless, we observed a beneficial effect of tDCS on shift trials specifically in the emotion condition using (hypothesis driven) post hoc tests. Prior studies have shown that tDCS enhances WM for non-emotional information (e.g., Fregni et al, 2005) and cognitive processes for emotional information (e.g., Boggio et al, 2007;Vanderhasselt et al, 2013).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Healthy individuals who tend to use reappraisal to overcome negative affect in daily life were behaviorally faster and exerted more dACC activity when inhibiting a response to negative in favor of positive information (compared to inhibiting a positive in favor of a negative response) (Vanderhasselt, Baeken, Van Schuerbeek, Luypaert, & De Raedt, 2013).…”
Section: A Cognitive Framework Of Depression and The Relationship Witmentioning
confidence: 98%