2014
DOI: 10.1037/xlm0000005
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

“Heads or tails?”—A reachability bias in binary choice.

Abstract: When asked to mentally simulate coin tosses, people generate sequences that differ systematically from those generated by fair coins. It has been rarely noted that this divergence is apparent already in the very 1st mental toss. Analysis of several existing data sets reveals that about 80% of respondents start their sequence with Heads. We attributed this to the linguistic convention describing coin toss outcomes as "Heads or Tails," not vice versa. However, our subsequent experiments found the "first-toss" bi… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

3
16
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 23 publications
(20 citation statements)
references
References 25 publications
3
16
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Nevertheless, in each experiment, the proportion of choices that were frame-consistent was greater when the frame-consistent choice was presented first. These non-significant differences are in the same direction as that reported by Schwitzgebel and Cushman (2015) , and also in line with the reachability bias ( Bar-Hillel et al, 2014 ). Moreover, if we combine our samples, the effect approaches significance using a one-tailed test: 63.1% choose the frame-consistent choice when that option was presented initially versus 56.1% when that option was presented last, p = 0.057 by Fisher’s exact one-sided test.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 88%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Nevertheless, in each experiment, the proportion of choices that were frame-consistent was greater when the frame-consistent choice was presented first. These non-significant differences are in the same direction as that reported by Schwitzgebel and Cushman (2015) , and also in line with the reachability bias ( Bar-Hillel et al, 2014 ). Moreover, if we combine our samples, the effect approaches significance using a one-tailed test: 63.1% choose the frame-consistent choice when that option was presented initially versus 56.1% when that option was presented last, p = 0.057 by Fisher’s exact one-sided test.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 88%
“…We manipulated option order as recommended by Fagley and Miller (1997) . In most studies, the certain option is presented first, yet Bar-Hillel et al (2014) have shown a “reachability bias” in response-option selection favoring the option presented first. Kühberger and Gradl (2013) found that although option order had no effect on choice in the positive-frame condition, a greater proportion of participants in the negative-frame condition chose the uncertain option when it was presented after the certain option than when it was presented initially, contrary to the reachability bias.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Participants received NIS 2 (about $0.40) voucher to the campus cafeteria for every toss landing on tails . We chose tails in Hebrew to correspond with heads in English in reachability (Bar‐Hillel, Peer, & Acquisti, ). Participants were screened to be native Hebrew speakers.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This order effect is realized by at least two mechanisms (Bar‐Hillel, ; Rodway, Schepman, & Thoma, ). The first mechanism is physical reachability (Bar‐Hillel, Peer, & Acquisti, ). The idea is that ceteris paribus, items located in the most reachable location are the most likely to be selected.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%