1975
DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.32.6.951
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Heads I win, tails it's chance: The illusion of control as a function of the sequence of outcomes in a purely chance task.

Abstract: Attributions in a purely chance task (predicting coin tosses) were studied as a function of either a descending, ascending, or random sequence of outcomes and as a function of whether the subject performed the task himself or observed another subject performing the task. A primary effect was predicted, that is, that early successes would induce a skill orientation towards the task. The prediction was supported. Subjects in the descending condition rated themselves as significantly better at predicting the outc… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

10
357
0
11

Year Published

1982
1982
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
5
5

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 661 publications
(421 citation statements)
references
References 10 publications
10
357
0
11
Order By: Relevance
“…The identification of losses with a new state sЈ that must be differentiated from an original state s may potentially explain the effect of "hindsight bias" (Custer, 1984;Langer & Roth, 1975;Wagenaar, 1988), in which gamblers analyze their losses and explain them away by noting what went wrong and why they should have known they would lose. Similarly, this theory explains the "illusion of control," in which gamblers believe they can control statistical situations (Custer, 1984;Elster, 1999;Wagenaar, 1988), as a misclassification of a single statistical situation into multiple differentiable situations (i.e., as a problem with the situation-categorization component).…”
Section: Problem Gamblingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The identification of losses with a new state sЈ that must be differentiated from an original state s may potentially explain the effect of "hindsight bias" (Custer, 1984;Langer & Roth, 1975;Wagenaar, 1988), in which gamblers analyze their losses and explain them away by noting what went wrong and why they should have known they would lose. Similarly, this theory explains the "illusion of control," in which gamblers believe they can control statistical situations (Custer, 1984;Elster, 1999;Wagenaar, 1988), as a misclassification of a single statistical situation into multiple differentiable situations (i.e., as a problem with the situation-categorization component).…”
Section: Problem Gamblingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Anticipation of success is prone to biases linked to the desirability of the outcome (Seybert and Bloomfield 2009;Vosgerau 2010;Windschitl et al 2010) or to perceived but non-existent control over the outcome (Goodman and Irwin 2006;Langer and Roth 1975). At the same time, information supporting desirable outcomes is perceived to be more credible than information supporting undesirable outcomes (Barber et al 2009;Gordon et al 2005).…”
Section: H1bmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Unfortunately, the research does not make it possible to differentiate the extent to which subjects are seeking meaning per se (which we regard as a type of control) from the extent to which they are seeking the positive thoughts and positive mood that often accompany the achievement of meaning. Some authors, especially Bulman and Wortman (1977) and Langer et al (1975), emphasize the former; others, especially Lazarus and Launier (1978), emphasize the latter. In the light of Lazarus and Launier's emphasis, it is ironic that they provided some of the most eloquent descriptions of secondary control dynamics.…”
Section: Interpretive Controlmentioning
confidence: 99%