1985
DOI: 10.1177/154193128502900621
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Head-Up Display (HUD) Utility, II: Runway to Hud Transitions Monitoring Eye Focus and Decision Times

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
30
1

Year Published

1993
1993
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 44 publications
(33 citation statements)
references
References 1 publication
2
30
1
Order By: Relevance
“…In spite of their general success however, some disconcerting issues have been raised regarding the clutter caused by overlapping imagery (Oppitek, 1973) and in one simulator evaluation, this clutter was assumed to be responsible for pilots failing to notice a plane taxiing onto the runway, on the final approach to landing (Fischer, Haines, and Price, 1980). Weintraub and his colleagues (Weintraub and Ensing, 1992;Weintraub, Haines, and Randle, 1985) have noted that an objective evaluation of the costs and benefits of HUDs must be based upon a controlled evaluation of the three dimensions by which HUDS differ from conventional instrumentation: their location, the fact that they are collimated to optical infinity, and the fact that their symbology is often different from that of conventional aircraft instruments. Most HUD evaluations have not applied these controls.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…In spite of their general success however, some disconcerting issues have been raised regarding the clutter caused by overlapping imagery (Oppitek, 1973) and in one simulator evaluation, this clutter was assumed to be responsible for pilots failing to notice a plane taxiing onto the runway, on the final approach to landing (Fischer, Haines, and Price, 1980). Weintraub and his colleagues (Weintraub and Ensing, 1992;Weintraub, Haines, and Randle, 1985) have noted that an objective evaluation of the costs and benefits of HUDs must be based upon a controlled evaluation of the three dimensions by which HUDS differ from conventional instrumentation: their location, the fact that they are collimated to optical infinity, and the fact that their symbology is often different from that of conventional aircraft instruments. Most HUD evaluations have not applied these controls.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Several reports indicate that optically overlaid information cannot be processed in parallel [22][23][24]. Others have reported that there is a time cost associated with cognitive switching among the environment and the [25][26][27].…”
Section: Effect Of Information Overlay On Performancementioning
confidence: 99%
“…This could be either through attentional capture, also called cognitive tunneling (Fischer & Haines, 1980;Weintraub, Haines, & Randle, 1985;Wickens & Long, 1995), or merely by the fact that light transmission through the HUD is not 100%. A commercial HUD will let about 85% to 90% of the incoming light pass through the glass plate.…”
Section: Hud Use In Commercial Flight Operationsmentioning
confidence: 98%