2010
DOI: 10.1177/0956797610370158
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

General Magnitude Representation in Human Infants

Abstract: Demonstrations of reciprocal behavioral interactions among the dimensions of space, number, and time, along with evidence of shared neural mechanisms in posterior parietal cortex, are consistent with a common representational code for general magnitude information. Although much recent speculation has concerned the developmental origins of a system of general magnitude representation, direct evidence in preverbal infants is lacking. Here we show that 9-month-olds transfer associative learning across magnitude … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

18
234
1
1

Year Published

2010
2010
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 303 publications
(254 citation statements)
references
References 36 publications
18
234
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Third, whereas adults can form translations between any dimensions of magnitude (38), it is not clear whether infants are similarly flexible. Current findings indicate that infants can map three pairs of extensive dimensions-number and space (16,17), time and space (17,22), and number and time (17)-and one pair of intensive dimensions-brightness and loudness (39)-but they may fail to relate dimensions belonging to different categories-loudness and length (22) or number and brightness (40). Finally, it is not clear whether the abilities shown by newborn infants are unique to our species or shared by other animals.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 78%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Third, whereas adults can form translations between any dimensions of magnitude (38), it is not clear whether infants are similarly flexible. Current findings indicate that infants can map three pairs of extensive dimensions-number and space (16,17), time and space (17,22), and number and time (17)-and one pair of intensive dimensions-brightness and loudness (39)-but they may fail to relate dimensions belonging to different categories-loudness and length (22) or number and brightness (40). Finally, it is not clear whether the abilities shown by newborn infants are unique to our species or shared by other animals.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 78%
“…The propensity to represent numerical magnitudes by the lengths of line segments (number lines) is a widespread phenomenon not only across cultures and species but also over human development. Human infants (16,17), children (18,19), educated human adults (18,20), and uneducated adults living in remote cultures (11) map numbers onto corresponding line lengths. Similarly, spatial-temporal mappings show the universal effects of one of these dimensions on the other, both in human adults (14) and in adult monkeys (21).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…De Hevia and colleagues also found further evidence for an inherited preference for relating numerosity to space in infants. Eight-month-old infants perceive a correspondence between the increasing or decreasing numerosity of geometrical shapes and an increasing or decreasing sequence of spatial lengths (lines) (de Hevia & Spelke, 2010; see also Lourenco & Longo, 2010, for similar results). By the same age, infants show SNARC-congruent attentional shifts when presented with small and large numerosities (dots).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 87%
“…One recent study showed that 9-mo-olds were equally likely to transfer an arbitrary, experimentally learned magnitude-to-texture association from one dimension (e.g., number) to another dimension (size or duration) (59). In addition, 9-mo-olds can readily learn pairs of positively (but not negatively) correlated line lengths and tone durations (60), suggesting that infants at least can represent an abstract "more-than" and "less-than" representation that applies to both dimensions.…”
Section: For Review)mentioning
confidence: 99%