2010
DOI: 10.1016/j.lingua.2010.06.006
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Frequency and economy in the acquisition of variable word order

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
29
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

3
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 33 publications
(33 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
(36 reference statements)
4
29
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In the Tromsø acquisition corpus (Anderssen 2006), the high subject position is attested 81% (1351/1667) in relevant utterances in the adult data. The three children in the corpus are sensitive to this distinction from early on (Westergaard 2008a, Anderssen & Westergaard 2010, Westergaard 2011, typically producing DP subjects in the low position (following negation) and pronominal subjects high (preceding negation), as illustrated in (5) To conclude this section, the data from children's spontaneous production of word order variation, both at the clausal and the phrasal level, show that they produce both options from early on. Furthermore, they generally produce the two word orders in appropriate contexts.…”
Section: Westergaard Vangsnes and Lohndal 2012mentioning
confidence: 98%
“…In the Tromsø acquisition corpus (Anderssen 2006), the high subject position is attested 81% (1351/1667) in relevant utterances in the adult data. The three children in the corpus are sensitive to this distinction from early on (Westergaard 2008a, Anderssen & Westergaard 2010, Westergaard 2011, typically producing DP subjects in the low position (following negation) and pronominal subjects high (preceding negation), as illustrated in (5) To conclude this section, the data from children's spontaneous production of word order variation, both at the clausal and the phrasal level, show that they produce both options from early on. Furthermore, they generally produce the two word orders in appropriate contexts.…”
Section: Westergaard Vangsnes and Lohndal 2012mentioning
confidence: 98%
“…The children’s overuse of the low subject position could of course simply be due to the children at an early stage producing a higher number of examples with pronominal subjects that happen to be focused than they do at later stages. However, in Anderssen & Westergaard (2010), a detailed investigation of the relevant examples in context shows that the children’s early production is in fact non‐target‐consistent. Thus, their production at period 2 (as well as period 3 for Ina) corresponds to a real preference for low subjects at an early stage 4 .…”
Section: Subject Positions In Norwegian Child Language Datamentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In this paper I first review some child language data on the different subject positions in , from Westergaard (2008a) and Anderssen & Westergaard (2010). As the findings deviate somewhat from the expected distribution in the target language as expressed in (8), it is pertinent to find out exactly what input children are typically exposed to in the acquisition process.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These data are especially significant, in light of a wealth of work on acquisition of V2 languages which views [XP Non-Subject [V Fin ]] as a defining characteristic of the linguistic output of speakers with a V2 grammar and a crucial cue for V2 acquisition (Poeppel and Wexler, 1993:14;Lightfoot, 1995Lightfoot, :40, 1999Yang, 2000:113;Westergaard, 2008:72;Anderssen andWestergaard, 2010:2575;Van Kampen, 2010:273). 6 The data are worth further scrutiny in light of observations by Sitaridou (2011:164) for Old Spanish and Kaiser (2002:134) and Rinke and Meisel (2009) for Old French that XP-V-S orders are insufficiently attested in the textual record to motivate a V2 analysis.…”
Section: The Matrix Left Peripherymentioning
confidence: 99%