2008
DOI: 10.1037/a0013025
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Fluency heuristic: A model of how the mind exploits a by-product of information retrieval.

Abstract: Boundedly rational heuristics for inference can be surprisingly accurate and frugal for several reasons. They can exploit environmental structures, co-opt complex capacities, and elude effortful search by exploiting information that automatically arrives on the mental stage. The fluency heuristic is a prime example of a heuristic that makes the most of an automatic by-product of retrieval from memory, namely, retrieval fluency. In 4 experiments, the authors show that retrieval fluency can be a proxy for real-w… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

14
166
1

Year Published

2013
2013
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 173 publications
(181 citation statements)
references
References 59 publications
(82 reference statements)
14
166
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Word frequency is an indicator of a word's fluency; more frequently occurring words are easier to access than less frequently occurring words (e.g., Hertwig, Herzog, Schooler, & Reimer, 2008;McClelland & Rumelhart, 1981). Though low frequency words may be more likely to elicit TOTs than high frequency words (e.g., Burke et al, 1991), if participants are inclined to infer from a TOT state a heightened state of accessibility for the unretrieved target relative to when no TOT state is present, they may infer from the presence of a TOT state a greater likelihood of the unretrieved target being of higher frequency.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Word frequency is an indicator of a word's fluency; more frequently occurring words are easier to access than less frequently occurring words (e.g., Hertwig, Herzog, Schooler, & Reimer, 2008;McClelland & Rumelhart, 1981). Though low frequency words may be more likely to elicit TOTs than high frequency words (e.g., Burke et al, 1991), if participants are inclined to infer from a TOT state a heightened state of accessibility for the unretrieved target relative to when no TOT state is present, they may infer from the presence of a TOT state a greater likelihood of the unretrieved target being of higher frequency.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Likewise, metric accuracy was poor (Mdn OME ϭ 0.71, IQR OME ϭ 0.34). It is also noteworthy that among all pairs of variables in Table 1, recognition times and knowledge ratings are correlated most highly (Mdn r S ϭ Ϫ.57, IQR r S ϭ .29), which should not be surprising as both have been used as measures of familiarity in the past (e.g., Brown & Siegler, 1992;Hertwig et al, 2008).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…Footnote 1); (b) the recognition cue in a given domain has a low validity (Pachur & Hertwig, 2006;Pohl, 2006); and (c) a low retrieval fluency of the recognized item in a given pair signals that the recognition cue might not be predictive in that particular instance (Marewski, Gaissmaier, et al, 2010;Marewski & Schooler, 2011). This last condition presupposes that the assessment of the speed with which the recognized pair member is processed-a potential index of the criterion (Hertwig, Herzog, Schooler, & Reimer, 2008;Schooler & Hertwig, 2005)-is used as a cue to evaluate the applicability of the RH.…”
Section: Ffh Framework: the Recognition Heuristic (Rh)mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations