2015
DOI: 10.1177/0049124115591016
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

First Equals Most Important? Order Effects in Vignette-Based Measurement

Abstract: Standard-Nutzungsbedingungen:Die Dokumente auf EconStor dürfen zu eigenen wissenschaftlichen Zwecken und zum Privatgebrauch gespeichert und kopiert werden.Sie dürfen die Dokumente nicht für öffentliche oder kommerzielle Zwecke vervielfältigen, öffentlich ausstellen, öffentlich zugänglich machen, vertreiben oder anderweitig nutzen.Sofern die Verfasser die Dokumente unter Open-Content-Lizenzen (insbesondere CC-Lizenzen) zur Verfügung gestellt haben sollten, gelten abweichend von diesen Nutzungsbedingungen die in… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
85
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 82 publications
(87 citation statements)
references
References 80 publications
1
85
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Some studies find that context effects are greater among people with weak attitudes (Schwarz et al 1991). Auspurg and Jäckle (2012) find that individuals with weak attitudes are more susceptible to order effects of characteristics within a vignette. Other studies find no association between context and attitude strength (see review by Tourangeau et al 2003).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 76%
“…Some studies find that context effects are greater among people with weak attitudes (Schwarz et al 1991). Auspurg and Jäckle (2012) find that individuals with weak attitudes are more susceptible to order effects of characteristics within a vignette. Other studies find no association between context and attitude strength (see review by Tourangeau et al 2003).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 76%
“…Interestingly, we do find that attribute level overlap and color coding had an impact on the estimated preference structure: Attribute level overlap reduced the emphasis that respondents place on the first and last attribute in the DCE. Without overlap, the first and last attribute typically receive the most attention from respondents, see, for example, Chrzan (), Kjær, Bech, Gyrd‐Hansen, and Hart‐Hansen () and Auspurg and Jäckle (), and the introduction of overlap subsequently forces respondents to take all attributes into account and thus more equally divides their attention over all included attributes. Intensity color coding increased the distance between “slight” and “moderate” problems, which are notoriously difficult to distinguish in the Dutch translation of the EQ‐5D and more clearly separated when color coding was used. Given that illogical preference reversals were observed in the DCE results for three of the five dimensions in the official Dutch EQ‐5D valuation study for precisely these levels (Versteegh et al, ) and because color coding had no impact on the overall range of the preference estimates, we are inclined to consider this a beneficial outcome of color coding. Highlighting of differences reduced the emphasis placed on the moderate levels and increased the emphasis placed on the more extreme levels.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…3 Vignette methodologists find that respondents perform well with ten vignettes per person and no more than twelve randomized dimensions per vignette (Auspurg and Jäckle 2015;Sauer et al 2011). With this design, the number of vignettes and dimensions is small enough to avoid biases that might jeopardize validity and reliability, such as cognitive overload, learning effects, and order effects, but the number of vignettes is large enough to explore within-and between-individual variation in the outcome variables of interest.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%