2016
DOI: 10.3389/fevo.2016.00013
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Female Song Rate and Structure Predict Reproductive Success in a Socially Monogamous Bird

Abstract: Bird song is commonly regarded as a male trait that has evolved through sexual selection. However, recent research has prompted a re-evaluation of this view by demonstrating that female song is an ancestral and phylogenetically widespread trait. Species with female song provide opportunities to study selective pressures and mechanisms specific to females within the wider context of social competition. We investigated the relationship between reproductive success and female song performance in the New Zealand b… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

0
17
0
1

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 24 publications
(21 citation statements)
references
References 68 publications
0
17
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Contrary to this prediction, we observed females singing only in response to a simulated intrusion from a rival female, supporting a function in resource and/or mate defense. Consistent with an adaptive, territorial function, females of multiple temperate species sing most often during aggressive interactions with rival females (Langmore 1998), and recent evidence suggests that female song rate is positively related to fitness in some species (Cain et al 2015, Brunton et al 2016.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 70%
“…Contrary to this prediction, we observed females singing only in response to a simulated intrusion from a rival female, supporting a function in resource and/or mate defense. Consistent with an adaptive, territorial function, females of multiple temperate species sing most often during aggressive interactions with rival females (Langmore 1998), and recent evidence suggests that female song rate is positively related to fitness in some species (Cain et al 2015, Brunton et al 2016.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 70%
“…Intrasexual resource competition offers a mechanism by which a link between an ornamental trait and fecundity could emerge: if ornamented females, for example, are more likely to win competitions with other females for access to resources that enhance fecundity, such as high‐quality territories or food resources, males that prefer ornamented females will have more fecund mates (Tobias, Montgomerie, & Lyon, ). Emerging evidence supports the idea that, as suggested by multiple reviews covering the function and evolution of female ornamentation (Lyon & Montgomerie, ; Tobias et al., ; Webb et al., ; West‐Eberhard, ), female visual and vocal ornaments are often used in female–female competition over resources related to reproductive success (Brunton et al., ; Cain et al., ; Crowhurst, Zanollo, Griggio, Robertson, & Kleindorfer, ; Kraaijeveld et al., ; Krieg & Getty, ; Murphy et al., ,b; Pryke, ; Stankowich & Caro, ; Watson & Simmons, ). For example, female black swans ( Cygnus atratus ) with more curled feathers are more likely to win female–female agonistic interactions, and number of curled feathers also predicts the ability to maintain territory ownership, which leads to higher offspring survivorship (Kraaijeveld et al., ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 80%
“…For example, female black swans (Cygnus atratus) with more curled feathers are more likely to win female-female agonistic interactions, and number of curled feathers also predicts the ability to maintain territory ownership, which leads to higher offspring survivorship (Kraaijeveld et al, 2004). In New Zealand bellbirds (Anthornis melanura), females use song to defend breeding territories against other females, and female song rate predicts the number of young fledged (Brunton, Evans, Cope, & Ji, 2008;Brunton et al, 2016). Similar links among female song rate, female territorial defense against other females, and female offspring production occur in house wrens (Troglodytes aedon;Krieg & Getty, 2016) and superb fairy-wrens (Malurus cyaneus; Cain et al, 2015).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Similarly, in species in which females have dull plumage, there has been repeated, independent selection on feather color in both males and females (Dale et al, 2015; Hofmann et al, 2008; Price et al, 2014), with females experiencing more evolutionary plumage change than males in some lineages (Price et al, 2014). Even though losses of female song and plumage appear to contribute considerably to dimorphism in signaling traits, the processes and conditions leading to these changes within individual species have not received much attention, particularly in species where female song is rare (Brunton et al, 2016; Kleindorfer et al, 2016a).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In contrast, relatively few studies have found relationships between female visual signals and fitness proxies, but see: (Amundsen et al, 1997; Bulluck et al, 2017; Cain & Ketterson, 2011; Jawor et al, 2004; Pap et al, 2019). Likewise, many studies have examined the fitness correlates of male song (Catchpole & Slater, 2003; Gil & Gahr, 2002), but only a handful have looked at how female song impacts fitness (Brunton et al, 2016; Cain et al, 2015; Kleindorfer et al, 2016b; Krieg & Getty, 2016a). Although integrated approaches can best untangle the relative contributions of selection on males and females to these signaling modalities (Hebets et al, 2016; Riebel et al, 2019), researchers have seldom included both visual and acoustic phenotypes from both sexes within the same studies.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%