1966
DOI: 10.1121/1.1909991
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Feedback and Psychophysical Variables in Signal Detection

Abstract: 144 observers, divided into eight groups of 18 each, were run in a two-alternative, temporal, forced-choice auditory-signal-detection task. At each of two signal intensities, four levels of information feedback were used. No feedback (NF); correct feedback on every trial (F100), on three-fourths (F75), or half (F50) of the trials, with incorrect feedback on remaining trials. The results were that (a) NF and F100 led to higher probability of correct responding P(C) than either F75 or F50 for both signal intensi… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
15
1

Year Published

1969
1969
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 38 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
1
15
1
Order By: Relevance
“…If the old/new decision criterion varied in its position from trial to trial, feedback could help minimize this criterion variance by informing subjects more quickly and directly about the appropriateness of a chosen location. This would, in turn, elevate performance because trial-to-trial changes in the criterion location reduce the observed accuracy of the observer (Carterette, Friedman, & Wyman, 1966;Friedman, Carterette, & Nakatani, 1968;Treisman & Williams, 1984). Although plausible, neither of these hypotheses was supported by the results.…”
Section: Experiments 1 Unbiased Feedback Versus Nonfeedback (Old/new Rcontrasting
confidence: 53%
“…If the old/new decision criterion varied in its position from trial to trial, feedback could help minimize this criterion variance by informing subjects more quickly and directly about the appropriateness of a chosen location. This would, in turn, elevate performance because trial-to-trial changes in the criterion location reduce the observed accuracy of the observer (Carterette, Friedman, & Wyman, 1966;Friedman, Carterette, & Nakatani, 1968;Treisman & Williams, 1984). Although plausible, neither of these hypotheses was supported by the results.…”
Section: Experiments 1 Unbiased Feedback Versus Nonfeedback (Old/new Rcontrasting
confidence: 53%
“…The effect of feedback often depends on the way in which it is combined with other variables. For example, several studies have reported that (correct) feedback facilitates detection performance when the discrimination is a difficult one, but actually impedes performance on an easy discrimination (Carterette, Friedman, & Wyman, 1966;Carterette & Wyman, 1962;Gundy, 1961).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…On the contrary, a tradition of examining the selection of nonverbal sounds also developed throughout this period. For example, a great deal of research has been performed to delineate sensitivity to quiet or brief auditory signals (e.g., Carterette, Friedman, & Wyman, 1966;Creelman, 1959;Green, McKey, & Licklider, 1959;Jeffress, 1967). Results obtained from many such experiments have established that the accuracy of detection of a pure tone embedded in noise (this sound is usually referred to as the probe) is best when the frequency expected by the listener matches the frequency of the sound actually presented, with performance declining as the frequency difference between the probe and expectation increases (e.g., Dai, Scharf, & Buus, 1991;Huggins, 1952;Johnson & Hafter, 1980;Macmillan & Schwartz, 1975;Penner, 1972;Scharf, Quigley, Aoki, Peachey, & Reeves, 1987;Schlauch & Hafter, 1991;Sorkin, Pastore, & Gilliom, 1968).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%