Prospective Memory 2019
DOI: 10.4324/9781351000154-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Fate of suspended and completed prospective memory intentions

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
9
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

2
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 1 publication
0
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The finished phase of the experimental procedure has been referred to in many different ways such as "test block" (Walser et al, 2012), "Phase 2" (Pink & Dodson, 2013;Scullin et al, 2012), "completed phase" (Anderson & Einstein, 2017), "finished PM phase" (Bugg et al, 2016) and "Block 2" . Here, we decided to use an active/finished terminology to more clearly delineate phases of the experiment in which an intention is "active" and should be pursued compared to phases in which the intention is "finished" and should not be pursued (Bugg & Streeper, 2019). (A) Schematic illustration of prototypical experimental procedures of paradigms to assess aftereffects of completed intentions.…”
Section: Paradigms To Assess Aftereffects Of Completed Intentionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The finished phase of the experimental procedure has been referred to in many different ways such as "test block" (Walser et al, 2012), "Phase 2" (Pink & Dodson, 2013;Scullin et al, 2012), "completed phase" (Anderson & Einstein, 2017), "finished PM phase" (Bugg et al, 2016) and "Block 2" . Here, we decided to use an active/finished terminology to more clearly delineate phases of the experiment in which an intention is "active" and should be pursued compared to phases in which the intention is "finished" and should not be pursued (Bugg & Streeper, 2019). (A) Schematic illustration of prototypical experimental procedures of paradigms to assess aftereffects of completed intentions.…”
Section: Paradigms To Assess Aftereffects Of Completed Intentionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…What are the mechanisms behind such aftereffects, and what causes us to slow down our ongoing-task performance while pursuing an intention in the first place? While aftereffects of completed intentions have been suggested to occur due to continued spontaneous retrieval of intentions after their completion, little is known about the underlying retrieval processes (Bugg & Streeper, 2019 ). Similarly, while several studies consistently reported costs to an ongoing task while actively pursuing a PM intention, the mechanisms underlying these costs are still a subject of discussion (Anderson et al, 2019 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Notably, since both spontaneous retrieval processes could manifest in slowed responses and/or commission errors (e.g., Bugg & Streeper, 2019 ; Möschl et al, 2020 ), neither of these subprocesses can be distinguished directly by discrete measures like RT aftereffects or commission errors alone. Instead, however, they should become distinguishable in continuous behavioral measures that allow an investigation of the genesis of a reflexively triggered response over time.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To examine PM commission errors in lab settings, the “finished paradigm” was developed (Scullin, Bugg, & McDaniel, 2012 ; for review, see Bugg & Streeper, 2019 ; cf. Walser, Fischer, & Goschke, 2012 ; for a related but distinct habitual PM paradigm, see Einstein, McDaniel, Smith, & Shaw, 1998 ; McDaniel, Bugg, Ramuschkat, Kliegel, & Einstein, 2009 ).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In striking contrast, 56% of participants in Experiment 1 and 46% of participants in Experiment 2 made a commission error in the zero-target condition. The authors concluded that PM intentions that remain unfulfilled are more accessible than intentions that are fulfilled, which is referred to as the intention fulfillment effect (Bugg & Streeper, 2019 ).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%