1998
DOI: 10.3758/bf03199238
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Familiarity and relatedness: Effects on social learning about foods by Norway rats and Mongolian gerbils

Abstract: In recent experiments in which the social influences on feeding in Mongolian gerbils were investigated, observer gerbils acquired food preferences from conspecific demonstrators only if the demonstrators and observers were either related or familiar. Even then, the effects of demonstrator gerbils on observers' food choices lasted less than 24 h. In similar experiments with Norway rats, the familiarity/relatedness of demonstrators and observers had little effect on social learning, and the demonstrators' influe… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
16
0

Year Published

2001
2001
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
2
1

Relationship

3
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 28 publications
(16 citation statements)
references
References 13 publications
0
16
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In contrast, rats in the present study were able to engage in unrestricted species-typical social behavior for 21 hr/day in the home cage, such as pinning, chasing, and rolling, as well complex behavioral sequences of deception and surprise attacks (Panksepp et al, 1984). These behaviors play a critical role in the development of normal social relationships in socially housed rats (Hol et al, 1999; Van den Berg et al, 1999; Von Frijtag et al, 2002), and likely play a modulatory role in social learning tasks in which responding depends on the degree of familiarity between the subjects (see, for example, Choleris et al, 1998; Galef et al, 1998; Kavaliers et al, 2005). …”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In contrast, rats in the present study were able to engage in unrestricted species-typical social behavior for 21 hr/day in the home cage, such as pinning, chasing, and rolling, as well complex behavioral sequences of deception and surprise attacks (Panksepp et al, 1984). These behaviors play a critical role in the development of normal social relationships in socially housed rats (Hol et al, 1999; Van den Berg et al, 1999; Von Frijtag et al, 2002), and likely play a modulatory role in social learning tasks in which responding depends on the degree of familiarity between the subjects (see, for example, Choleris et al, 1998; Galef et al, 1998; Kavaliers et al, 2005). …”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Social learning about foraging has been shown in a wide range of species (Danchin et al, 2004) and when an individual is faced with choosing between two unfamiliar food options, it may benefit initially by choosing the option chosen by other animals. Indeed, naïve rats prefer the flavour that matches that of food consumed by an experienced individual (Galef et al, 1998(Galef et al, , 1984. In this way social learning enables the observer to consume a known, safe food while avoiding a potentially poisonous, unknown food.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…This one-trial procedure for inducing a learned flavor preference, described in detail in Galef (2002), provides a simple, noninvasive, costeffective way to produce robust changes in appetitive behavior not only in Norway rats (Rattus norvegicus), but also in house mice (Mus domesticus; Valsecchi & Galef, 1989), Mongolian gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus; Galef et al, 1998), pine voles (Microtus pinetorum; Solomon, Yaeger, & Beeler, 2002), spiny mice (Acomys cahirinus; McFadyen-Ketchum & Porter, 1989), and Belding's ground squirrel (Spermophilus beldingii; Peacock & Jenkins, 1988).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%