2017
DOI: 10.1080/00221325.2017.1361377
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Facial Expressions and the Ability to Recognize Emotions from the Eyes or Mouth: A Comparison Between Children and Adults

Abstract: The authors sought to contribute to the literature on the ability to recognize anger, happiness, fear, surprise, sadness, disgust, and neutral emotions from facial information. They aimed to investigate if-regardless of age-this pattern changes. More specifically, the present study aimed to compare the difference between the performance of adults and 6- to 7-year-old children in detecting emotions from the whole face and a specific face region, namely the eyes and mouth. The findings seem to indicate that, for… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

3
17
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 19 publications
(20 citation statements)
references
References 43 publications
3
17
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Specifically, the mouth has been consistently found to be both sufficient and necessary to discriminate happiness (Calder and Jansen, 2005;Nusseck et al, 2008;Bombari et al, 2013;Beaudry et al, 2014;Maher et al, 2014). A smiling mouth has been widely considered as a salient and distinctive facial feature in previous studies (Calvo and Nummenmaa, 2016;Guarnera et al, 2017;Wegrzyn et al, 2017;Calvo et al, 2018), a finding that supports the feature-based process of happiness recognition (Beaudry et al, 2014). The eye region, which includes the eyes and eyebrows, has been identified as important, sufficient, and necessary for discriminating sad expressions (Calvo et al, 2006;Calvo and Nummenmaa, 2008;Wegrzyn et al, 2017;Ikeda, 2020), but the eyes and eyebrows are seldom explored separately.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Specifically, the mouth has been consistently found to be both sufficient and necessary to discriminate happiness (Calder and Jansen, 2005;Nusseck et al, 2008;Bombari et al, 2013;Beaudry et al, 2014;Maher et al, 2014). A smiling mouth has been widely considered as a salient and distinctive facial feature in previous studies (Calvo and Nummenmaa, 2016;Guarnera et al, 2017;Wegrzyn et al, 2017;Calvo et al, 2018), a finding that supports the feature-based process of happiness recognition (Beaudry et al, 2014). The eye region, which includes the eyes and eyebrows, has been identified as important, sufficient, and necessary for discriminating sad expressions (Calvo et al, 2006;Calvo and Nummenmaa, 2008;Wegrzyn et al, 2017;Ikeda, 2020), but the eyes and eyebrows are seldom explored separately.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…In general, humans use the mouth more than the eyes to both signal and discriminate facial expressions [ 9 11 ]. The recognition patterns of emotions from faces are generally similar among children, adults, and the elderly [ 12 , 13 ].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, although even newborns are able to produce facial expressions [56], the ability to recognize specific emotions from facial expressions increases with age [57, 58]. Previous studies reported that, among the basic facial expressions, the emotions that are best recognized (from an actor’s full face display) are happiness, anger, and disgust, followed by fear, with sadness being more difficult to recognize [5961]. More specifically, research has shown that, by 5 years of age, children are as sensitive as adults to displays of happiness, [62], and from 8 to 11 years, they recognize happy, angry, and disgust expressions more easily than those showing fear and sadness [62, 63].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%