2009
DOI: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2008.06.010
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Extended recruitment efforts minimize attrition but not necessarily bias

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

8
78
0
2

Year Published

2009
2009
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
5
1
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 80 publications
(89 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
8
78
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Despite the higher response rates, however, almost none of the estimates from the survey changed significantly. Similar outcomes have been observed numerous times, for example by Curtin et al (2005), Haring et al (2009), andIngen et al (2009). While there are several possible explanations for the lack of an effect on the estimates, these examples point out gaps in our understanding of the effects of data collection efforts on biases.…”
Section: Brick: Unit Nonresponse and Weighting Adjustmentssupporting
confidence: 74%
“…Despite the higher response rates, however, almost none of the estimates from the survey changed significantly. Similar outcomes have been observed numerous times, for example by Curtin et al (2005), Haring et al (2009), andIngen et al (2009). While there are several possible explanations for the lack of an effect on the estimates, these examples point out gaps in our understanding of the effects of data collection efforts on biases.…”
Section: Brick: Unit Nonresponse and Weighting Adjustmentssupporting
confidence: 74%
“…Details on the SHIP study design and data have been published. 19,20 In brief, from the 212,157 inhabitants living in the area, a representative sample of 7008 subjects aged between 20 and 79 years was selected using population registries where all German inhabitants are registered. Only individuals with German citizenship and main residency in the study area were included.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Survey data may be biased because people who complete surveys tend to be significantly different to those who do not, often on variables central to the study questions [e.g., 8,9]. One method of dealing with this bias is to assume that late responders are similar to nonresponders and make adjustments based on the attitudes or characteristics of late participants [10]. This method is consistent with the continuum of resistance model [11,12], which suggests that people who respond in the first contact lie at one end of a continuum and non-respondents at the other end.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Late participants are seen to be "more like" non participants in that they would never have participated had they not received a reminder [13]. However, a number of studies have thrown doubt on the continuum of resistance model, suggesting that non-participants are not more similar to late participants than they are to early participants [10,12,14,13]. For example, Haring et al [10] found that late respondents were generally younger, single, better educated, and employed than early respondents but found no support for the hypothesis that late respondents were similar to non-respondents.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation