1995
DOI: 10.1111/j.2044-835x.1995.tb00675.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Explaining young children's difficulty on the false belief task: Representational deficits or context‐sensitive knowledge?

Abstract: This study compared two explanations for the poor performance of 3-year-olds on the false belief task: an inability to represent false beliefs and context-sensitive problem solving schemas. Experiment 1 tested preschoolers' understanding of the appearancereality distinction and of false beliefs. Some of the results were: an age difference was found on the appearancereality assessment but not on the false belief assessment; 3-year-olds performed better on the false belief assessment than on the appearance-reali… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
9
0
1

Year Published

1997
1997
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 18 publications
(11 citation statements)
references
References 13 publications
1
9
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Children solved deceptive box tasks more easily than unexpected transfer tasks (in Experiment 2, but not in Experiment 1), were unaffected in the latter type of task by whether the transfer in location was accidental or intentionally deceitful (cf. Dalke, 1995), and showed skill at solving false belief tasks about another person earlier than about themselves. These findings are, at first glance, problematic, as they seemingly conflict with the findings of a recent meta‐analysis of factors influencing children's success on false belief tasks (Wellman et al , 2001).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Children solved deceptive box tasks more easily than unexpected transfer tasks (in Experiment 2, but not in Experiment 1), were unaffected in the latter type of task by whether the transfer in location was accidental or intentionally deceitful (cf. Dalke, 1995), and showed skill at solving false belief tasks about another person earlier than about themselves. These findings are, at first glance, problematic, as they seemingly conflict with the findings of a recent meta‐analysis of factors influencing children's success on false belief tasks (Wellman et al , 2001).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Previous findings conflict with regard to whether (1) children are likely to follow their deception with correct responses to false belief questions, and whether (2) a deceptive context actually enhances their false belief performance compared to a standard context (e.g., Brooks, Samuels, & Frye, 1998;Charman & Karpf, 1996;Dalke, 1995;Hala et al, 1991;Ruffman et al, 1993;Sodian et al, 1991;Sullivan & Winner, 1991. A second aim, then, was to illuminate these issues further by examining children's understanding of false belief in both deceptive and nondeceptive contexts.…”
Section: Studymentioning
confidence: 93%
“…We will illustrate this possibility with respect to children's deception. In recent years, deception has been examined as a potential index of early false belief understanding (e.g., Chandler, Fritz, & Hala, 1989;Charman & Karpf, 1996;Dalke, 1995;Hala, Chandler, & Fritz, 1991;Peskin, 1992Peskin, ,1996Ruffman, Olson, Ash, & Keenan, 1993;Russell, Mauthner, Sharpe, & Tidswell, 1991;Sodian, 1991;Sodian, Taylor, Harris, & Perner, 1991;. Full-fledged deception implicates an understanding of false belief because the very point of deception is to create a false belief in the mind of another for some ulterior purpose.…”
Section: Deception and False Beliefmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…We will illustrate this possibility with respect to children's deception. In recent years, deception has been examined as a potential index of early false belief understanding (e.g., Chandler, Fritz, & Hala, 1989;Charman & Karpf, 1996;Dalke, 1995;Hala, Chandler, & Fritz, 1991;Peskin, 1992Peskin, ,1996Ruffman, Olson, Ash, & Keenan, 1993;Russell, Mauthner, Sharpe, & Tidswell, 1991;Sodian, 1991;Sodian, Taylor, Harris, & Perner, 1991;. Full-fledged deception implicates an understanding of false belief because the very point of deception is to create a false belief in the mind of another for some ulterior purpose.…”
Section: Deception and False Beliefmentioning
confidence: 99%