Oxford Handbooks Online 2017
DOI: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199338801.013.22
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Experimental Designs in the Study of Offender Decision Making

Abstract: Research on offender decision making has utilized experimental designs and has often coupled these strong designs with the use of hypothetical vignettes that describe specific offending circumstances for the would-be offender to consider. In some cases, these studies have experimentally manipulated situational elements of the imagined setting. In others, researchers have experimentally manipulated the context in which the participants make the decision. Other researchers have utilized randomized designs with b… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
2

Relationship

0
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 2 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…they are limited in their ability to capture the more visceral and emotional aspects involved in real-world offending, which commonly occurs during "hot" and altered states of mind (Exum and Bouffard 2010:581). Secondly, a 10-to 15-line narrative may not reflect the complex reality of real-life situations and realistically incorporate important nuances of social experience (e.g., Bouffard and Niebuhr 2017;Christian, Edwards, and Bradley 2010;Ditto et al 2006;Parkinson and Manstead 1993). Finally, the scenario method relies on the ability of people to imagine themselves in a hypothetical situation and is not sensitive to individual differences in this ability (Collett and Childs 2011;Van Gelder, Martin, et al 2017).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…they are limited in their ability to capture the more visceral and emotional aspects involved in real-world offending, which commonly occurs during "hot" and altered states of mind (Exum and Bouffard 2010:581). Secondly, a 10-to 15-line narrative may not reflect the complex reality of real-life situations and realistically incorporate important nuances of social experience (e.g., Bouffard and Niebuhr 2017;Christian, Edwards, and Bradley 2010;Ditto et al 2006;Parkinson and Manstead 1993). Finally, the scenario method relies on the ability of people to imagine themselves in a hypothetical situation and is not sensitive to individual differences in this ability (Collett and Childs 2011;Van Gelder, Martin, et al 2017).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Ideally, the empirical study of such a person-environment interaction is based on situational data about the behavioral outcomes of particular person-place convergences, i.e., data that spatiotemporally link the person, the setting, and the action. In principle, such data can be obtained in various ways, including laboratory and field experiments (Farrington et al 2020), retrospective reporting in surveys (Wikström et al 2012), experimental scenario methods (Bouffard & Niebuhr 2017, Exum & Bouffard 2010, and analysis of process-generated data such as video footage from public surveillance cameras (Ejbye-Ernst et al 2020).…”
Section: Testing Situational Hypothesesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Hence, we advocate a theory-guided focus on person-environment interactions at the research design stage rather than checking only for treatment effect heterogeneity after (quasi)experimental work has been conducted. Finally, hypothetical scenarios have become a major method to test hypotheses about the interplay of personal and setting characteristics (Bouffard & Niebuhr 2017, Exum & Bouffard 2010, Nagin & Paternoster 1993, Wikström et al 2012. Compared to lab experiments, scenarios allow the representation of any kind of crime, but they do not capture actual behavior.…”
Section: Testing Situational Hypothesesmentioning
confidence: 99%