2017
DOI: 10.1111/mbe.12145
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Encouraging Spatial Talk: Using Children's Museums to Bolster Spatial Reasoning

Abstract: Longitudinal spatial language intervention studies have shown that greater exposure to spatial language improves children's performance on spatial tasks. Can short naturalistic, spatial language interactions also evoke improved spatial performance? In this study, parents were asked to interact with their child at a block wall exhibit in a children's museum. Some parents were instructed to emphasize formal shape terms, others to emphasize spatial goals, and some were not provided scripts. Children were presente… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
35
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 46 publications
(45 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
1
35
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Children’s spatial word production (e.g., “left/right”, “by/next”, “middle”) predicts performance on tasks involving those spatial relations (Hermer-Vazquez et al, 2001; Miller et al, 2016; Simms & Gentner, 2008). Similarly, children who hear more spatial words from their caregivers in unstructured play (Pruden et al, 2011) or in museum-based interactions (Polinsky, Perez, Grehl, & McCrink, 2017) tend to produce more spatial words themselves, and perform better on spatial tasks. Based on these findings, theorists proposed that, as children acquire or gain experience using spatial words, they become better at using language to encode relevant spatial features, enhancing their spatial performance (e.g., Pruden et al, 2011).…”
Section: Investigations Of Language Effects On Spatial Skillsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Children’s spatial word production (e.g., “left/right”, “by/next”, “middle”) predicts performance on tasks involving those spatial relations (Hermer-Vazquez et al, 2001; Miller et al, 2016; Simms & Gentner, 2008). Similarly, children who hear more spatial words from their caregivers in unstructured play (Pruden et al, 2011) or in museum-based interactions (Polinsky, Perez, Grehl, & McCrink, 2017) tend to produce more spatial words themselves, and perform better on spatial tasks. Based on these findings, theorists proposed that, as children acquire or gain experience using spatial words, they become better at using language to encode relevant spatial features, enhancing their spatial performance (e.g., Pruden et al, 2011).…”
Section: Investigations Of Language Effects On Spatial Skillsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, an experiment using random assignment showed how children's shape learning could be enhanced. More recently, a brief intervention randomly assigning parents visiting a children's museum showed that instructing parents to talk about shapes, rather than about another topic, speeded the children's subsequent puzzle completion (Polinsky et al, 2017).…”
Section: Preschool Childrenmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, parents of 5-year-olds who discussed spatial relationships between images that portrayed different viewpoints of the same scene had children with better spatial skills (Szechter & Liben, 2004). The importance of spatial language use has also been documented in a museum setting (Polinsky, Perez, Grehl, & McCrink, 2017), where an experimental manipulation of instruction to parents led to the use of more spatial language during a spatial construction task at an exhibit. Improvements in post-test scores on a spatial task were related to the amount of spatial language children used.…”
Section: Language and Early Spatial Developmentmentioning
confidence: 99%