2009
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.03.004
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Encoding of others’ beliefs without overt instruction

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
38
1

Year Published

2012
2012
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
10

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 39 publications
(40 citation statements)
references
References 20 publications
1
38
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Some researchers have suggested that this general approach can also be applied in the study of theory-of-mind (Cohen & German, 2009; Apperly & Butterfill, 2009). They suggest that there is a quick heuristic system for automatically calculating others’ mental states, alongside a slower more effortful system.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some researchers have suggested that this general approach can also be applied in the study of theory-of-mind (Cohen & German, 2009; Apperly & Butterfill, 2009). They suggest that there is a quick heuristic system for automatically calculating others’ mental states, alongside a slower more effortful system.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…An important question is whether or not the costly nature of ÔTheory of MindÕ (ToM) processes observed in past studies is an integral part the inferences themselves, making them different or delayed with respect to inferences about behaviour not drawing on mental state information. This issue has recently been addressed in terms of whether ToM inferences are made automatically-or whether they only come into play under certain circumstances (as in Apperly et al, 2006;Back & Apperly, 2010;Cohen & German, 2009). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Schneider's work suggests this Type 1 mindreading of belief-like states occurs without any conscious awareness (Schneider et al, 2012a), and is unintentional and uncontrollable (Schneider et al, 2014). Other automaticity studies (e.g., Cohen & German, 2009) use a similar setup, having participants explicitly engage in a non-mindreading, object-tracking task. But they instead use a verbal task: they measure participants' reaction times in responding to questions about the object's location at the end of the video (e.g., "It is true that the object is in the location on the left") vs. questions about the agent's belief (e.g., "She thinks the object is in the location on the left").…”
Section: Applications Of Type 1 Minimal Mindreading Of Belief-like Smentioning
confidence: 99%