2013
DOI: 10.1002/bdm.1801
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Emotions in Advice Taking: The Roles of Agency and Valence

Abstract: Recently, advice taking has received attention in decision-making research, and some studies suggest that emotions may play a role in this process. Yet a clear account of how emotions influence advice taking is lacking. The current research introduces a parsimonious explanation by suggesting that such effects can be predicted on the basis of two emotion dimensions: valence (positivity or negativity) and agency (self-focused versus other-focused). In five experiments with different emotion inductions and differ… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
40
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 46 publications
(46 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
4
40
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We also computed the commonly used weight on advice measure ( f − i )/( a − i ) and its different variants (e.g. De Hooge et al, ; Gino & Moore, ; Harvey & Fischer, ; See et al, ; Yaniv, ). The results again provided support for hypothesis 2.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…We also computed the commonly used weight on advice measure ( f − i )/( a − i ) and its different variants (e.g. De Hooge et al, ; Gino & Moore, ; Harvey & Fischer, ; See et al, ; Yaniv, ). The results again provided support for hypothesis 2.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Gino, Brooks, and Schweitzer () studied the role of anxiety in increasing receptiveness. De Hooge, Verlegh, and Tzioti () developed a parsimonious account of how the effect of specific emotions on advice‐taking behavior could be determined based on their valence (positive or negative) and agency (self‐focused or other‐focused). For example, gratitude (positive, other‐focused) would lead to more receptiveness than anger (negative, other‐focused), while pride (positive, self‐focused) would lead to less receptiveness than shame (negative, self‐focused).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, although fear in Study 2 is presumed to be a negative, other‐caused emotion, in reality, fear could also be caused by the situation itself. Similarly, one could feel angry with another person or with oneself (De Hooge et al, ). Therefore, the agency of a single emotion might differ across situations.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Participants followed the method of Study 2. This time, they recalled a situation in which they felt positive due to their own behavior (positive self‐caused condition), or due to the behavior of other people (positive other‐caused condition), or they felt negative due to their own behavior (negative self‐caused condition), or due to the behavior of other people (negative other‐caused condition) (De Hooge et al, ). In the control condition, participants recalled a normal weekday.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some work has reported effective results of praise in modifying behavior (Cooke et al 2011;Everett et al 2005). One of the central reasons proposed in the literature is that praise evokes a positive emotional response, which in turn motivates people to continue doing what they were praised for doing (De Hooge, Verlegh, and Tzioti 2013;Delin and Baumeister 1994;Geers, Handley, and McLarney 2003). Yet other research has reported lower compliance with praise (Befera and Barkley 1985;Birch, Marlin, and Rotter 1984;Dowling, Slep, and O'Leary 2009;Jones, Sloane, and Roberts 1992), suggesting that praise may be detrimental to behavior change because it can delay or stifle the development of autonomous individuals (Cannella 1986) or lead to uncomfortable feelings such as embarrassment (Delin and Baumeister 1994).…”
Section: Conceptual Background How Praising and Scolding Mobilize Actionmentioning
confidence: 99%