2014
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0096254
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Elimination Behavior of Shelter Dogs Housed in Double Compartment Kennels

Abstract: For animals in confinement housing the housing structure has tremendous potential to impact well being. Dogs in animal shelters are often housed in one of two types of confinement housing – single kennels and rooms or double compartment kennels and rooms most often separated by a guillotine door. This study examines the effect of housing on the location of elimination behavior in dogs housed in double compartment kennels were the majority of the dogs were walked daily. One side of the kennel contained the food… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
9
0
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 10 publications
0
9
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…One solution may be to prevent the dogs from entering the back portion by designing kennels without compartments, or closing the door to the back compartment (most kennels have a divider between compartments for ease of cleaning). However, this solution may present welfare concerns because dogs use both compartments for different purposes: the front for toileting and the back for eating and resting (Wagner, Newbury, Kass, & Hurley, ). Another solution may be to evaluate a compound stimulus of the visual presentation of a person together with an auditory cue that may be presented only once when a visitor enters the shelter.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One solution may be to prevent the dogs from entering the back portion by designing kennels without compartments, or closing the door to the back compartment (most kennels have a divider between compartments for ease of cleaning). However, this solution may present welfare concerns because dogs use both compartments for different purposes: the front for toileting and the back for eating and resting (Wagner, Newbury, Kass, & Hurley, ). Another solution may be to evaluate a compound stimulus of the visual presentation of a person together with an auditory cue that may be presented only once when a visitor enters the shelter.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For comparisons between the UCCR of control group dogs and sheltered dogs after adoption, day 3 and day 12 after adoption (for both n = 7) were compared to the averaged data of two UCCR outcomes (12 days apart) for control dogs in homes without novelty experiences ( n = 21). No significant differences were found between UCCR at day 3 and 12 after adoption compared to those of control dogs (independent samples one-sided t -tests, day 3 vs averaged control dogs: estimated mean difference [ratio] = 1.12, 95% CI = 0.86—∞, t 33 = 0.73, p = 0.235; day 12 vs averaged control dogs: estimated mean difference [ratio] = 1.12, 95% CI = 0.87—∞, t 34 = 0.77, p = 0.224).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Seis abrigos mantinham 100% dos seus cães com pelagem limpa, e oito abrigos, acima de 80%. Wagner et al (2014) mostraram a probabilidade de 72 e 77% dos cães de abrigos defecarem e urinarem, respectivamente, no lado oposto à cama, mostrando a forte preferência dos cães em expressarem o comportamento de eliminação em espaços distintos da alimentação e da área de descanso. Recintos pequenos ou lotados não permitem esse comportamento, comprometendo o bem-estar dos animais e a limpeza do pelame.…”
Section: Resultsunclassified