2011
DOI: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2011.02.002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Domain-specific Rapid Automatized Naming deficits in children at risk for learning disabilities

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
15
0
2

Year Published

2013
2013
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 31 publications
(20 citation statements)
references
References 40 publications
3
15
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…In congruence with e.g., van der Sluis et al (2004) and Pauly et al (2011), the results of this study support the hypothesis of the comorbid group as an additive combination of RD and MD. They might indicate that RD and MD have a distinct cognitive profile (Landerl, Fussenegger, Moll, & Willburger, 2009).…”
Section: Cognitive Risk Factorssupporting
confidence: 92%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In congruence with e.g., van der Sluis et al (2004) and Pauly et al (2011), the results of this study support the hypothesis of the comorbid group as an additive combination of RD and MD. They might indicate that RD and MD have a distinct cognitive profile (Landerl, Fussenegger, Moll, & Willburger, 2009).…”
Section: Cognitive Risk Factorssupporting
confidence: 92%
“…Underadditivity would be the case: performance on behavioral inhibition tasks is the same in children with RD+MD, in children with RD and in children with MD. If the cognitive impairments in MD are independent of those in RD, then the RD+MD group would be the sum or the additive combination of the deficits in each pure group (Landerl et al, 2004;Pauly et al, 2011;van der Sluis et al, 2004;Willcutt, Pennington, Olson, Chhabildas, & Hulslander, 2005).…”
Section: Objectives and Research Questionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…No entanto, outras tarefas também podem ser encontradas na literatura. Essas outras tarefas, apesar de muito semelhantes às de Denckla e Rudel, diferem dessas na quantidade de itens em uma mesma tarefa (15 itens em Landerl & Willbuger, 2010;25 itens em Zhou et al, 2014;36 itens em Logan, Schatschneidere, & Wagner, 2011;40 itens em Shapiro, Carroll, & Solity, 2013;48 em Clarke, Hulme, & Snowling, 2005) e/ou na quantidade de estímulos diferentes em cada tarefa (seis itens diferentes em Pauly, Linkersdörfer, & Lindberg, 2011, e Logan et al, 2011; 10 a 25 itens diferentes em Clarke et al, 2005; 20 itens em Cobbold, Passenger, & Terrel, 2003). Embora essas variações pudessem, a princípio, afetar a relação entre a nomeação seriada rápida e a leitura, a meta-análise realizada por Araújo et al (2015) revelou que nenhum desses aspectos (variação no tamanho da tarefa ou na quantidade de itens diferentes incluídos) teve um impacto significativo na magnitude da correlação entre a nomeação seriada rápida e a leitura.…”
unclassified
“…Similar tasks are closely associated with reading and spelling skill development, with reading achievement, and with a number of variables related to reading and spelling, and have shown sensitivity to specific learning disorder in reading (Crews & D'Amato, 2009;Korkman, Barron-Linnankoski, & Lahti-Nuuttila, 1999;Korkman, Kirk, & Kemp, 2007;Powell, Stainthorp, Stuart, Garwood, & Quinlan, 2007;Willburger, Fussenegger, Moll, Wood, & Landerl, 2008). Some studies suggest they are also related to mathematics skills, specific learning disorder-mathematics, and a number of other clinical conditions (McGrew & Wendling, 2010;Pauly et al, 2011;Willburger et al, 2008;Wise et al, 2008). In order to ensure sensitivity beyond very early grades, the tasks involve naming multiple dimensions simultaneously and alternating stimuli.…”
Section: Naming Speed Literacymentioning
confidence: 99%