2014
DOI: 10.1017/s0142716414000320
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Does type of modified output correspond to learner noticing of feedback? A closer look in face-to-face and computer-mediated task-based interaction

Abstract: This study examines if type of learner-modified output differentially demonstrates noticing and whether or not this relationship holds in both face-to-face (FTF) and synchronous computer-mediated chat (SCMC) environments. Twenty-four learners of Spanish as a foreign language interacted one-on-one with an interlocutor in the FTF and the SCMC modes, during which they received corrective feedback. After each interaction, learners participated in a mode-specific stimulated recall session to measure their noticing … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
42
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 81 publications
(44 citation statements)
references
References 46 publications
2
42
0
Order By: Relevance
“…With respect to the effectiveness of corrective feedback prompting output, the results support various studies showing a facilitative role for modified output in subsequent L2 development (e.g., Loewen, 2005;Gurzynski-Weiss & Baralt, 2015;McDonough & Mackey, 2006). Indeed, the effect sizes obtained here (third-person singular -s: d = 0.70, 95% CI [0.23, 1.57]; possessive determiners: d = 0.51, 95% CI [−0.17, 1.11]) approached the mean betweengroup values (i.e., d = 0.83, 95% CI [0.56, 1.10]) found in Lyster and Saito's (2010) meta-analysis of seven primary classroom studies.…”
Section: Implicit Input-providing Versus Implicit Output-prompting Cosupporting
confidence: 83%
“…With respect to the effectiveness of corrective feedback prompting output, the results support various studies showing a facilitative role for modified output in subsequent L2 development (e.g., Loewen, 2005;Gurzynski-Weiss & Baralt, 2015;McDonough & Mackey, 2006). Indeed, the effect sizes obtained here (third-person singular -s: d = 0.70, 95% CI [0.23, 1.57]; possessive determiners: d = 0.51, 95% CI [−0.17, 1.11]) approached the mean betweengroup values (i.e., d = 0.83, 95% CI [0.56, 1.10]) found in Lyster and Saito's (2010) meta-analysis of seven primary classroom studies.…”
Section: Implicit Input-providing Versus Implicit Output-prompting Cosupporting
confidence: 83%
“…As Yoshida (2010) states, "It may be necessary for teachers to keep in mind that the real nature of the learners' problems might not be quite what they understand them to be" (p. 311). Future studies could examine this in detail, particularly given the fact that investigations of instructors' CF intention(s) and students' perception(s) attest that instructors and learners are not always on the same page when it comes to CF (Gurzynski-Weiss & Baralt, 2014, 2016Mackey et al, 2000).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Theoretical Rationale for Oral CF Corrective feedback promotes language development in several ways, a claim that is upheld in cognitive-interactionist (e.g., Gass & Mackey, 2007;Long, 1996;Schmidt, 2001;Swain, 2005) and social (e.g., Duff, 2010;Friedman, 2009;Nassaji & Swain, 2000) approaches alike. While engaged in meaningful communication, CF can direct learners to notice mismatches between their production and the target language (Gurzynski-Weiss & Baralt, 2014;Mackey et al, 2000) and encourage learners to restructure their interlanguage (Braidi, 1995;Lyster & Mori, 2006), test out hypotheses of the correct form via modified output (Egi, 2010;Gurzynski-Weiss & Baralt, 2016) and develop linguistically to be more in line with their immediate interlocutor (Aljaafreh & Lantolf, 1994;Lantolf & Poehner, 2007. In FL settings, CF is seen as particularly critical, given that learners have limited opportunities to become aware of their emerging language system and how it may deviate from the target language; in most cases, FL learners' only opportunity to receive CF is in the classroom (Philp & Tognini, 2009).…”
Section: Review Of the Literaturementioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…From a language pedagogic perspective, it has been claimed that the distinct characteristics of written SCMC increase the salience of the linguistic input during L2 chat thereby guiding a learner's attention to linguistic form and supporting uptake and intake of L2 forms (Smith, 2005). First, most people type more slowly than they speak, which results in a slower pace of turn-taking during SCMC than in spoken interaction (Gurzynski-Weiss & Baralt, 2015), resulting in what Beauvois (1992) refers to it as a "conversation in slow motion" (p. 455). Second, the output of a chat conversation remains visible on the screen, which also increases the potential salience of lexical items and grammatical structures.…”
Section: Salience In Sla and Scmcmentioning
confidence: 99%