2015
DOI: 10.17851/2237-2083.23.2.335-361
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Does the mental lexicon exist?

Abstract: Abstract:One of the central and most intriguing components of language processing to researchers is the mental lexicon. The term was used for the first time by Ann Triesman in 1961 and we still do not have clear answers on how it is structured and how much information it contains, or even if there is something to be called a mental lexicon. For some time, the mental lexicon has been compared to a mental dictionary both storing and organizing word knowledge; however, they are surely different in structure and q… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
2
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(2 citation statements)
references
References 16 publications
0
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Somewhat surprisingly, many models of semantic memory and the mental lexicon employ network‐like structures, if not the explicit use of a network representation (e.g., Collins & Loftus, 1975; De Deyne, Navarro, Perfors, Brysbaert, & Storms, 2018; Landauer & Dumais, 1997; Miller, 1995; Quillian, 1967; Smith, Shoben, & Rips, 1974), suggesting that these types of representations are intuitive for capturing the structure of word–word similarity relationships. However, to date, there is little consensus on the exact nature of the underlying representation of the mental lexicon (e.g., what linguistic relations to include) in part because the ability to quantify such a large, complex system (i.e., tens of thousands of words) has remained somewhat elusive (Jones, Willits, et al, 2015; Sousa & Gabriel, 2015; Steyvers & Tenenbaum, 2005).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Somewhat surprisingly, many models of semantic memory and the mental lexicon employ network‐like structures, if not the explicit use of a network representation (e.g., Collins & Loftus, 1975; De Deyne, Navarro, Perfors, Brysbaert, & Storms, 2018; Landauer & Dumais, 1997; Miller, 1995; Quillian, 1967; Smith, Shoben, & Rips, 1974), suggesting that these types of representations are intuitive for capturing the structure of word–word similarity relationships. However, to date, there is little consensus on the exact nature of the underlying representation of the mental lexicon (e.g., what linguistic relations to include) in part because the ability to quantify such a large, complex system (i.e., tens of thousands of words) has remained somewhat elusive (Jones, Willits, et al, 2015; Sousa & Gabriel, 2015; Steyvers & Tenenbaum, 2005).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although such errors may arise due to disruptions in the process of lexical retrieval, in this paper, we demonstrate that it is important to also consider how the structure (or organization) of the mental lexicon influences that process. Our current approach is driven by a key tenet within the cognitive sciences that our understanding of word retrieval is necessarily incomplete without a careful examination of the role that mental lexicon structure plays in language processing (Jones, Willits, & Dennis, 2015; Sousa & Gabriel, 2015; Steyvers & Tenenbaum, 2005). In this paper, we investigated the role of mental lexicon structure on speech error production by persons with aphasia using techniques from network science, an interdisciplinary field of study focusing on the structure and processes of complex systems (e.g., telecommunication networks, brain networks, semantic networks; Lewis, 2011; Newman, Barabási, & Watts, 2006), to quantify mental lexicon structure (see reviews Baronchelli, Ferrer‐i‐Cancho, Pastor‐Satorras, Chater, & Christiansen, 2013; Siew, Wulff, Beckage, & Kenett, 2019).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%