2016
DOI: 10.1080/0163853x.2015.1137445
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Do Causal and Concessive Connectives Guide Emotional Expectancies in Comprehension? A Double-Task Paradigm Using Emotional Icons

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
16
1

Year Published

2018
2018
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 19 publications
(20 citation statements)
references
References 29 publications
3
16
1
Order By: Relevance
“…They also scored worse for connectives that conveyed a concessive relation than those conveying conditional or consequence relations. These findings support the assumption that causal relations are not only easier to process than concessive ones (Murray 1997;Morera et al 2017) but are also mastered better by native speakers. In addition, our native participants scored significantly better for causal relations than for additive ones.…”
Section: Scores On the Connectives Tasksupporting
confidence: 84%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…They also scored worse for connectives that conveyed a concessive relation than those conveying conditional or consequence relations. These findings support the assumption that causal relations are not only easier to process than concessive ones (Murray 1997;Morera et al 2017) but are also mastered better by native speakers. In addition, our native participants scored significantly better for causal relations than for additive ones.…”
Section: Scores On the Connectives Tasksupporting
confidence: 84%
“…These were chosen because they imply various degrees of cognitive complexity. Based on previous studies (e.g., Murray 1997;Sanders et al 1992;Morera et al 2017) these relations can be roughly ordered along the following scale of cognitive complexity (left = least complex, right = most complex):…”
Section: Designmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A reflection of this circumstance can be seen in the fact that concessives are acquired rather late(Di Meola 1997;Spooren & Sanders 2008;Knoepke et al 2016) and are more difficult to process(Morera et al 2017;Xu et al 2018) and to understand (Köhne & Demberg 2012) than, say, causatives. Furthermore, primary school children have some problems using them(Becker & Musan 2014;Dragon et al 2015;Knoepke et al 2016).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Experimentally, these findings fit with the existing evidence that processing counter-argumentative relations is more demanding than processing causal relations, particularly in late integrative processes. Nevertheless, compared with previous research on the same semantic relations (Köhne & Demberg, 2013; Zunino, 2014, 2016, 2017), and with others focusing on concessive and causal ones (Morera et al, 2017; Xu et al, 2018), the current study has novel implications: the combination of two kinds of coherence, presented conjointly and marked by a phrasal connective unit containing a neuter pronoun in Spanish, revealed higher working memory and cognitive load demands.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 61%
“…Particular attention has been devoted to the distance of the preceding antecedent (e.g., Carpenter & Just, 1977; Çokal, Sturt, & Ferreira, 2018; Duffy & Rayner, 1990; Ehrlich & Rayner, 1983; Fukumura & van Gompel, 2012), and to the gender and syntactic ambiguity that may arise in some contexts (Kennison, 2003; Kennison, Fernandez, & Bowers, 2009; Kennison & Trofe, 2003; Sturt, 2003, 2013). At the same time, causal and counter-argumentative semantic inter-sentential relations have also been studied intensely from different theoretical perspectives and with differing methodological techniques (Köhne & Demberg, 2013; Morera, León, Escudero, & de Vega, 2017; Parodi, Julio, & Recio, 2018; Prandi, 2004; Recio, Nadal, & Loureda, 2018; Sanders et al, 1992, 1993; Xu, Chen, Panther, & Wu, 2018; Zunino, 2017). Yet, rarely do studies center on them together focusing on the binary procedural instruction they provide to the reader (referential and relational), particularly from a distinctive discourse-oriented approach and with eye-tracking techniques (e.g., Ackerman, 1986; Çokal et al, 2018; Koornneef & Sanders, 2013).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%