2018
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00939
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Distinct Patterns of Cognitive Conflict Dynamics in Promise Keepers and Promise Breakers

Abstract: On a daily basis, we see how different people can be in keeping or breaking a given promise. However, we know very little about the cognitive conflict dynamics that underlie the decision to keep or break a promise and whether this is shaped by inter-individual variability. In order to fill this gap, we applied an ecologically valid promise decision task with real monetary consequences for all involved interaction partners and used mouse tracking to identify the dynamic, on-line cognitive processes that underli… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

0
6
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 44 publications
0
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…MD was calculated as the maximum perpendicular deviation between the real and the theoretical trajectories—that is, straight line connecting the start- and end-point of the mouse movement—while the AUC was calculated as the geometric area between them (Freeman et al, 2011; Freeman & Ambady, 2010). High values associated with MD and AUC indicate larger deviations toward the unselected choice option and are used as a proxy for uncertainty and cognitive conflict (Calluso et al, 2018; Freeman et al, 2011). Intuitively, a straight line connecting the “start” button to the selected option implies no uncertainty (i.e., the mouse is directed to the preferred option with no hesitation), whereas deviations from the straight line imply uncertainty and cognitive conflict whose magnitude is a function of the extent of the deviation.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…MD was calculated as the maximum perpendicular deviation between the real and the theoretical trajectories—that is, straight line connecting the start- and end-point of the mouse movement—while the AUC was calculated as the geometric area between them (Freeman et al, 2011; Freeman & Ambady, 2010). High values associated with MD and AUC indicate larger deviations toward the unselected choice option and are used as a proxy for uncertainty and cognitive conflict (Calluso et al, 2018; Freeman et al, 2011). Intuitively, a straight line connecting the “start” button to the selected option implies no uncertainty (i.e., the mouse is directed to the preferred option with no hesitation), whereas deviations from the straight line imply uncertainty and cognitive conflict whose magnitude is a function of the extent of the deviation.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The statistical significance of the effects was assessed using mixed effects-models (Baayen et al, 2008; Bates et al, 2015; R Core Team, 2014) because they are able to account for between-subject differences, which are unrelated to the task design/experimental manipulation. Additionally, such approach represents a gold standard analysis when dealing with mouse kinematics data to account for participants’ basic motor properties (e.g., a subject can show higher trajectories’ curvature or can be faster/slower compared to another, independently from the decision itself) and was also employed in many previous mouse-tracking studies (Barca & Pezzulo, 2012; Calluso et al, 2018, 2019, 2020; O’Hora et al, 2013a, 2016; Quétard et al, 2016; Quinton et al, 2013; Tabatabaeian et al, 2015).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For instance, when evaluating tasty but unhealthy food, people experience a conflict between the short‐term satisfaction and the long‐term consequences of unhealthy eating (Gillebaart, Schneider, & De Ridder, 2016; Schneider et al, 2015; Schneider, Gillebaart, & Mattes, 2019). Similarly, in interpersonal behaviors, people experience a conflict when choosing between their desire to increase their payoff and their social duty to cooperate with others (Kieslich & Hilbig, 2014) or to keep their promises (Calluso, Saulin, Baumgartner, & Knoch, 2018). Because equity is an important value for people (Adams, 1965; Fehr & Schmidt, 1999), having to pay high costs for it may put decision makers in a difficult dilemma.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To assess the level of conflict, we used mouse tracking techniques. In recent years, mouse tracking techniques have been successfully used to study cognitive conflict (Freeman & Ambady, 2010) in different decisions domains, such as food choices (Gillebaart et al, 2016; Schneider et al, 2015; Schneider & Schwarz, 2017), temporal discounting (Stillman & Ferguson, 2019), and more complex interpersonal behaviors (Calluso et al, 2018; Kieslich & Hilbig, 2014). The advantage of these techniques is that they bypass self‐report, which can be prone to biases and social desirability.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…tinuously translates into movement. This view is clearly embodied by existing computational accounts of movement tracking processes, including attractor models (Frisch et al, 2015;Scherbaum et al, 2016;Spivey et al, 2005;Zgonnikov et al, 2017) and drift-diffusion models (Calluso et al, 2018;Wong et al, 2015). According to these models, a latent preference for the available option evolves until either a stable (attractor models) or an extreme enough (diffusion models) point of preference for one of the options is reached.…”
Section: The Mapping Between Cognitive Process and Movementmentioning
confidence: 99%