2023
DOI: 10.1101/2023.02.28.530443
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Dimensionality and ramping: Signatures of sentence integration in the dynamics of brains and deep language models

Abstract: A sentence is more than the sum of its words: its meaning depends on how they combine with one another. The brain mechanisms underlying such semantic composition remain poorly understood. To shed light on the neural vector code underlying semantic composition, we introduce two hypotheses: First, the intrinsic dimensionality of the space of neural representations should increase as a sentence unfolds, paralleling the growing complexity of its semantic representation, and second, this progressive integration sho… Show more

Help me understand this report
View published versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

0
9
0

Year Published

2023
2023
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(11 citation statements)
references
References 117 publications
(94 reference statements)
0
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…There are good reasons to adopt this approach: the different regions of this network i) have similar functional response profiles, both with respect to their selectivity for language (e.g., [13][14][15]17,18 ) and their responses to linguistic manipulations (e.g., 21,152 ), and ii) exhibit highly correlated time courses during naturalistic cognition paradigms (e.g., 80,82,153,127,154,11 ). However, some functional heterogeneity has been argued to exist within the language network (e.g., 30,29,32,151,155,156 ). Future efforts using an approach like the one adopted here could perhaps discover functional differences within the language network (by searching for stimuli that would selectively drive particular regions within the network), as well as between the core LH language network and the RH homotopic areas and other language-responsive cortical, subcortical, and cerebellar areas.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There are good reasons to adopt this approach: the different regions of this network i) have similar functional response profiles, both with respect to their selectivity for language (e.g., [13][14][15]17,18 ) and their responses to linguistic manipulations (e.g., 21,152 ), and ii) exhibit highly correlated time courses during naturalistic cognition paradigms (e.g., 80,82,153,127,154,11 ). However, some functional heterogeneity has been argued to exist within the language network (e.g., 30,29,32,151,155,156 ). Future efforts using an approach like the one adopted here could perhaps discover functional differences within the language network (by searching for stimuli that would selectively drive particular regions within the network), as well as between the core LH language network and the RH homotopic areas and other language-responsive cortical, subcortical, and cerebellar areas.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Inspection of the average timecourse by cluster ( Figure 2E ) revealed three distinct response profiles (see Figure 2D for best representative electrodes from each cluster —’medoids’— chosen by the k-medoids algorithm). Cluster 1 (n=92 electrodes; range across participants: 5-34, Figure S1 ) is characterized by a relatively slow increase (build-up) of neural activity across the 8 words in the S condition (a pattern similar to the one reported by Fedorenko et al, 2016; Nelson et al, 2017; Desbordes et al, 2023; Woolnough et al, 2023; but see Discussion), and much lower activity for the W, J, and N conditions, with no difference between the J and N conditions ( Figure 2F ). Cluster 2 (n=67 electrodes; range across participants: 1-21, Figure S1 ) displays a quicker build-up of neural activity in the S condition that plateaus approximately 3 words into the sentence, a quick build-up of activity in the W condition that begins to decay after the third word, and a similar response to the J and N conditions as to the W condition with an overall lower magnitude.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 69%
“…We used intracranial recordings from patients with intractable epilepsy to investigate neural responses during language comprehension. Participants in Dataset 1 were presented with four types of linguistic stimuli that have been traditionally used to tease apart neural responses to word meanings and syntactic structure (Fedorenko et al, 2010(Fedorenko et al, , 2012(Fedorenko et al, , 2016Pallier et al, 2011;Shain, Kean et al, 2023;Desbordes et al, 2023; for earlier uses of this paradigm, see Mazoyer et al, 1993;Friederici et al, 2000;Humphries et al, 2001;Vandenberghe et al, 2002): sentences (S), lists of unconnected words (W), Jabberwocky sentences (J), and lists of unconnected nonwords (N) (Figure 1A,B, Methods, all stimuli are available at osf.io/xfbr8/). In each trial, 8 words or nonwords were presented on a screen serially and participants were asked to silently read them.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations