1988
DOI: 10.1007/bf00174698
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Differential effects of opioid and nonopioid analgesics on conditional discriminations in pigeons

Abstract: Under the fixed-consecutive-number schedule (FCN), pigeons were reinforced for responding eight or more times on one response key (work key), and then responding once on a second response key. In one component of this schedule, an external stimulus signalled the completion of the response requirement on the work key (FCN 8-SD), whereas no stimulus change was programmed under the other (FCN 8). Across a range of doses, the mu opioid agonist morphine, the kappa opioid agonist U50,488 and the opioid antagonist na… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...

Citation Types

0
2
0

Year Published

1989
1989
1998
1998

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(2 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
(30 reference statements)
0
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The behavioral effects of drugs have been shown to depend upon a number of nonpharmacological factors, including the schedule of reinforcement (e.g., Branch, 1990;Hoffman, Branch, & Sizemore, 1987;McAuley & Leslie, 1986), the degree of external stimulus control (e.g., Rees, Wood, & Laties, 1985), the species of the experimental subject (e.g., Picker & Dykstra, 1988;Picker, Heise, & Dykstra, 1987), its gender (van Haaren, Heinsbroek, Louwerse, & van de Poll, 1986), its experimental history (Urbain, Poling, Millam, & Thompson, 1978), and the nature of the event that maintains the behavior.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The behavioral effects of drugs have been shown to depend upon a number of nonpharmacological factors, including the schedule of reinforcement (e.g., Branch, 1990;Hoffman, Branch, & Sizemore, 1987;McAuley & Leslie, 1986), the degree of external stimulus control (e.g., Rees, Wood, & Laties, 1985), the species of the experimental subject (e.g., Picker & Dykstra, 1988;Picker, Heise, & Dykstra, 1987), its gender (van Haaren, Heinsbroek, Louwerse, & van de Poll, 1986), its experimental history (Urbain, Poling, Millam, & Thompson, 1978), and the nature of the event that maintains the behavior.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One potential source of concern when using an FCN or any traditional differentiation procedure as a baseline against which to evaluate drug effects is that drug-induced disruptions in the run distribution will produce correlated changes in reinforcement probability and rate, generally decreasing criterional run frequency and consequently reinforcement density. Increasing doses of a wide variety of drugs, including various opioid agonists and antagonists (e.g., Bronson & Moerschbaecher, 1987;Picker, Heise, & Dykstra, 1987), the nonopi-205 1991) 561, [205][206][207][208][209][210][211][212][213][214][215] NUMBER 2 (SEPrEMBER) oid analgesics clonidine and 1-nantradol (Picker & Dykstra, 1988), the anticonvulsants clonazepam, ethosuximide (Picker, Leibold, Endsley, & Poling, 1986a) valproic acid, phenytoin, phenobarbital, diazepam (Picker, Leibold, Endsley, & Poling, 1986b), methsuximide, and mephenytoin (Schlinger, Wilkenfield, & Poling, 1988), the stimulants amphetamine (Laties, 1972), methamphetamine, caffeine, and methylphenidate (Mechner & Latranyi, 1963), and the anticholinergic scopolamine (Laties, 1972) all decrease the relative frequency of runs reinforced under FCN procedures. These decreases in reinforcement density are potentially important because they have been implicated in modifying both acute (e.g., Smith & McKearney, 1977) and chronic (cf.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%