1984
DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1984.tb04414.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Development of Motor Co‐ordination by Normal Left‐handed Children

Abstract: of ANOVA Summary of ANOVA Summary of ANOVA Age (5,84), F=20.15, p

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

1985
1985
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 40 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 14 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In left-handers, the same mirror-opposed, proximal-distal handwriting conflict may bring about the left hand inverted (LI) handwriting posture— a posture adopted by the majority of left hand writers (McKeever, 2004). Left-handed children commonly learn to print with a noninverted posture—subsequently adopting the inverted mode in the transition from manuscript to cursive (Bryson & McDonald, 1984; Gaillard, 1992; Levander & Schalling, 1988; Peters, 1983; Peters & Pedersen, 1978; Rudel, Healey, & Denckla, 1984) although even in adulthood the noninverted posture is frequently used for printing (Herron, 1980). Phonological awareness seems to drive this progression as children who fail to advance from a parallel (viz., proximal) to an inverted posture are impaired in the phonetic decoding of print (Allen & Wellman, 1980) and read poorly (Bryson & MacDonald, 1984; Wellman & Allen, 1983).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In left-handers, the same mirror-opposed, proximal-distal handwriting conflict may bring about the left hand inverted (LI) handwriting posture— a posture adopted by the majority of left hand writers (McKeever, 2004). Left-handed children commonly learn to print with a noninverted posture—subsequently adopting the inverted mode in the transition from manuscript to cursive (Bryson & McDonald, 1984; Gaillard, 1992; Levander & Schalling, 1988; Peters, 1983; Peters & Pedersen, 1978; Rudel, Healey, & Denckla, 1984) although even in adulthood the noninverted posture is frequently used for printing (Herron, 1980). Phonological awareness seems to drive this progression as children who fail to advance from a parallel (viz., proximal) to an inverted posture are impaired in the phonetic decoding of print (Allen & Wellman, 1980) and read poorly (Bryson & MacDonald, 1984; Wellman & Allen, 1983).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This finding concurs with the report that mirror (overflow) movements did not correlate with WRAT s p e h g at age seven in the prospective Collaborative Perinatal Project of the National lnstitute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke (Nichols 1987). In fact, Denckla andRude1 (1977, 1978) have suggested that overflow may be related to hyperactivity rather than learning disability. None of the finger tasks correlated with any of the criterion writing measures within the first, second or third grades.…”
Section: Validitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The inverted (hooked) writing posture (LI) commonly develops during the transition from manuscript to cursive writing ( Peters & Pedersen, 1978 ;Peters, 1983Peters, , 1986Bryson & MacDonald, 1984 ;Rudel, Healey, & Denckla, 1984 ;Levander & Schalling, 1988 ;Gaillard, 1992 ) and is used by the majority of adult left-handers ( McKeever, 2004 ). It may represent a left hand-right eye counterpart to the right hand-left eye condition postulated herein to be foundational in the development of dyslexia.…”
Section: Left-hand Inverted Writersmentioning
confidence: 97%