1994
DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.30.6.869
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Development of infants' use of continuity cues in their perception of causality.

Abstract: Two experiments investigated the role of continuity cues in infants' perception of launching events as causal. Experiment 1 showed that 7-month-old infants can use spatial and temporal contiguity to perceive causality: Infants who were habituated to a causal event dishabituated to novel noncausal events, in which either spatial or temporal contiguity was violated, and those who were habituated to a noncausal event dishabituated to a novel causal but not a novel noncausal event. Experiment 2 showed that 10-mont… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

7
127
1

Year Published

1996
1996
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 115 publications
(135 citation statements)
references
References 24 publications
(74 reference statements)
7
127
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The inferences infants make about hidden objects (e.g., permanence, contact causality) have the same informational content as the output of spatiotemporal analysis of the perceptual array. For example, infants, like adults, perceive mechanical causality directly when the spatiotemporal conditions for Michotte's launching events are met (Michotte 1963, Leslie 1988, Oakes 1994, and it is exactly this sort of contact causality that is inferred in the hidden events of the Ball studies cited above (Ball 1973. For another example, infants and adults analyze self-generated motion and patterns of contingent reaction between entities as evidence for intentional, goal directed action (Gergely et al 1995, Watson 1979).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The inferences infants make about hidden objects (e.g., permanence, contact causality) have the same informational content as the output of spatiotemporal analysis of the perceptual array. For example, infants, like adults, perceive mechanical causality directly when the spatiotemporal conditions for Michotte's launching events are met (Michotte 1963, Leslie 1988, Oakes 1994, and it is exactly this sort of contact causality that is inferred in the hidden events of the Ball studies cited above (Ball 1973. For another example, infants and adults analyze self-generated motion and patterns of contingent reaction between entities as evidence for intentional, goal directed action (Gergely et al 1995, Watson 1979).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…During the first year of life, infants acquire a rich array of physical knowledge in a consistent order; 3-to 4-mo-old infants understand that the world is composed of bounded, unitary objects (10) that are continuous in time and space (11). By 5 mo of age, most infants are able to differentiate a liquid from a solid using motion cues and have expectations about how nonsolids behave and interact (12, 13); by 6 or 7 mo, they are sensitive to the causal roles of one object striking and launching another (14,15), and by 8 mo, they can determine which objects must be attached for a configuration to be stable under gravity (16). By 12 mo, they are sensitive to the rough location of an object's center of mass, relative to the edge of a supporting surface, needed for that support relationship to be stable (17).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…During the first year, infants can distinguish biological motion from nonbiological motion for both people and other mammals (Arterberry & Bornstein, 2002;Bertenthal, 1993), identify both rational and intentional actions (Csibra, Gergely, Biro, Koos, & Brockbank, 1999;Woodward, 1999), and reason about the physical interaction between objects such as causality (e.g., Leslie, 1982;Oakes, 1994). Infants also parse actions in events (e.g., Baldwin, Baird, Saylor, & Clark, 2001;Sharon & Wynn, 1998;Spelke, Born, & Chu, 1983;Wynn, 1996).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%