1985
DOI: 10.1016/s0163-6383(85)80019-9
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Development of infants' sensitivity for slow and rapid motions

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

2
34
1
3

Year Published

1992
1992
2013
2013

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 54 publications
(40 citation statements)
references
References 16 publications
2
34
1
3
Order By: Relevance
“…As noted earlier, the visual modality is highly responsive to spatial variations, and, as a result, the finding of successful discrimination of changes in the rate of motion was not surprising. In fact, sensitivity to motion appears to be present at birth (Kaufmann, Stucki, & Kaufmann-Hayoz, 1985;Kremenitzer, Vaughan, Kurtzberg, & Dowling, 1979;Volkmann & Dobson, 1976). Moreover, motion appears to be a fundamental property of the visual world, and motion-detection mechanisms are found at many phylogenetic levels where pattem-detection mechanisms are not available at all, or are very rudimentary (Nakayama, 1985).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…As noted earlier, the visual modality is highly responsive to spatial variations, and, as a result, the finding of successful discrimination of changes in the rate of motion was not surprising. In fact, sensitivity to motion appears to be present at birth (Kaufmann, Stucki, & Kaufmann-Hayoz, 1985;Kremenitzer, Vaughan, Kurtzberg, & Dowling, 1979;Volkmann & Dobson, 1976). Moreover, motion appears to be a fundamental property of the visual world, and motion-detection mechanisms are found at many phylogenetic levels where pattem-detection mechanisms are not available at all, or are very rudimentary (Nakayama, 1985).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…According to the modality appropriateness hypothesis, infants' responsiveness to components of auditory/visual compounds should be affected by the degree to which the stimulation in each modality matches the specialization of the respective modalities. On the basis of the fact that infants respond more to moving displays than to spatially static ones (Volkmann & Dobson, 1976;Wilcox & Clayton, 1968) and that they respond differentially to different rates of visual motion early in infancy (Aslin & Shea, 1990;Dannemiller & Freedland, 1991;Freedland & Dannemiller, 1987;Kaufmann, Stucki, & Kaufmann-Hayoz, 1985;Lewkowicz, 1992), it seems reasonable to expect that infants would have little difficulty in detecting changes in the temporal characteristics of a visual component of an auditory/visual compound stimulus when the visual component is spatially dynamic. The .…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Aslin and Shea (1990) found a 9-deg/sec threshold at 6 weeks and a 4-deg/sec threshold at 12 weeks for vertical motion. Thresholds for rotary motion were lower, 1.4 and 0.93 deg/sec for 1-and 3-month-olds, respectively; but these thresholds were still elevated in comparison with adult levels (Kaufmann, Stucki, & Kaufmann-Hayoz, 1985), as were thresholds for oscillating gratings (Hamer & Norcia, 1994). Wattam-Bell (1991, 1996a, 1996b has identified changes in motion sensitivity between 6, 8, and 15 weeks of age.…”
mentioning
confidence: 92%
“…The stimuli were moving because it is a well established fact that infants prefer moving displays to static ones (e.g. Kaufmann et al, 1985), i.e. because motion attracts the infants' attention, and apparently exerts a facilitating effect on the infants' performance in visual habituationdishabituation tasks (e.g.…”
Section: Stimulimentioning
confidence: 99%