2007
DOI: 10.1007/s00442-007-0862-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Detecting small environmental differences: risk-response curves for predator-induced behavior and morphology

Abstract: Most organisms possess traits that are sensitive to changes in the environment (i.e., plastic traits) which results in the expression of environmentally induced polymorphisms. While most phenotypically plastic traits have traditionally been treated as threshold switches between induced and uninduced states, there is growing evidence that many traits can respond in a continuous fashion. In this experiment we exposed larval anurans (wood frog tadpoles, Rana sylvatica) to an increasing gradient of predation risk … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
54
0

Year Published

2009
2009
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

3
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 65 publications
(60 citation statements)
references
References 91 publications
(95 reference statements)
6
54
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Cages in treatments without predators were similarly disturbed. Previous experimental work shows that the concentration of predator cue used in this experiment is sufficient to induce plastic responses (Schoeppner and Relyea, 2008). The first predator feeding was on 11 May (defined as experiment day 1) and the last feeding was on day 45, just before leopard frog tadpoles began to metamorphose.…”
Section: Research Articlementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Cages in treatments without predators were similarly disturbed. Previous experimental work shows that the concentration of predator cue used in this experiment is sufficient to induce plastic responses (Schoeppner and Relyea, 2008). The first predator feeding was on 11 May (defined as experiment day 1) and the last feeding was on day 45, just before leopard frog tadpoles began to metamorphose.…”
Section: Research Articlementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Adaptive phenotypic plasticity requires reliable signals, and many species appear to use signals that are associated with interaction intensity (Wiackowski and Staronska 1999, Van Buskirk and Arioli 2002, Relyea 2004, Schoeppner and Relyea 2008. For example, if a predator-prey pair is sensitive to the presence of a second predator in the system, then the intensity of their interactions and thus the dosage of cues released (by the primary predator) to induce the prey defense may diminish, thereby reducing the degree of inducible defense expression.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Conditioned and unconditioned hellbenders reduced their activity by 70.1% and 40.8%, respectively, when presented with kairomones from one largemouth bass, but we did not observe further changes in chamber activity after exposure to high concentrations of kairomones. A threshold response following the addition of one predator is similar among other amphibians [65,66]. Wood frog tadpoles (Rana sylvatica) reduce their activity by 38% when presented with a single predator, but then show no additional differences in activity when two, four, or six predators are presented [65].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 78%
“…A threshold response following the addition of one predator is similar among other amphibians [65,66]. Wood frog tadpoles (Rana sylvatica) reduce their activity by 38% when presented with a single predator, but then show no additional differences in activity when two, four, or six predators are presented [65]. Oppositely, pool frog tadpoles increase the proportion of inactive individuals by 22% when they are presented with a single caged predator, but have no additional changes when three more caged predators are added to the same holding tank [66].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 85%