2014
DOI: 10.1037/a0035660
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Delay of gratification by orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) in the accumulation task.

Abstract: There is considerable evidence indicating that chimpanzees can delay gratification for extended time intervals, particularly in the accumulation task in which food items accumulate within a participant's reach until the participant begins to consume them. However, there is limited evidence that other ape species might also exhibit this capacity, despite there being a number of similar studies indicating that nonape species (e.g., monkeys and birds) can delay gratification, but not for nearly as long as chimpan… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

2
18
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

4
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 31 publications
(20 citation statements)
references
References 40 publications
(48 reference statements)
2
18
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Second, the results indicated that delay maintenance was driven not by the number of items obtained but instead by the delay interval. This is an important outcome, as it matches other previous work using intertemporal choice tasks with monkeys (e.g., Evans et al 2014) and also the accumulation task with orangutans (Parrish et al, 2014), and this highlights that improving self-control in this species will likely progress better when attention is paid to delay intervals rather than food amounts. Third, and perhaps most impressively, we documented that this group of monkeys now showed much stronger self-control in a task that they had not performed well at in earlier.…”
Section: 1 Experimentssupporting
confidence: 85%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Second, the results indicated that delay maintenance was driven not by the number of items obtained but instead by the delay interval. This is an important outcome, as it matches other previous work using intertemporal choice tasks with monkeys (e.g., Evans et al 2014) and also the accumulation task with orangutans (Parrish et al, 2014), and this highlights that improving self-control in this species will likely progress better when attention is paid to delay intervals rather than food amounts. Third, and perhaps most impressively, we documented that this group of monkeys now showed much stronger self-control in a task that they had not performed well at in earlier.…”
Section: 1 Experimentssupporting
confidence: 85%
“…This test, called the accumulation task, was first used with human children (e.g., Toner and Smith, 1977) and was later adapted for use with nonhuman animals (Beran, 2002). Some species are quite successful with this task, particularly the great apes (Beran, 2002; Beran and Evans, 2006; Evans and Beran, 2007a; Parrish et al, 2014; Stevens, Rosati, Heilbronner, and Mühlhoff, 2011). Other species such as monkeys (e.g., Anderson, Kuroshima, and Fujita, 2010; Evans and Beran, 2007b) and African Grey Parrots (Vick, Bovet, and Anderson, 2010; but see Koepke, Gray, & Pepperberg, 2015) do not maintain the same degree of delay maintenance that is shown by apes in the accumulation task.…”
Section: 1 Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In a less commonly used self-control task, animals choose when to take an amount of food that grows over time as additional items are added to it one by one (e.g., Anderson et al 2010;Beran 2002;Beran and Evans 2006;Parrish et al 2014;Vick et al 2010). Such accumulation (ACC) tasks have been presented to fewer species, with a larger focus on nonhuman primates.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…This is particularly true for research using intertemporal choice tasks, in which subjects make a discrete smaller-sooner versus larger-later choice (e.g., Ainslie, 1974;Berns, Laibson, Loewenstein, 2007;Deluty, 1978;Green, Myerson, Holt, Slevin, & Estle, 2004;Logue, 1988;Logue & Chavarro, 1987;Navarick & Fantino, 1976;Rachlin & Green, 1972;Stevens, Hallinan, & Hauser, 2005;Stevens & Mühlhoff, 2012;Tobin, Chelonis, & Logue, 1993;van Haaren, van Hest, & van de Poll, 1988). However, nonhuman animals also have performed a variety of other tasks, including delay of gratification tasks (e.g., Anderson, Kuroshima, & Fujita, 2010;Beran, 2002;Beran & Evans, 2006;Beran, SavageRumbaugh, Pate, & Rumbaugh, 1999;Brucks, Soliani, Range, & Marshall-Pescini, 2017;Evans & Beran, 2007;Evans, Beran, Paglieri, & Addessi, 2012;Grosch & Neuringer, 1981;Hillemann, Bugnyar, Kotrschal, & Wascher, 2014;Koepke, Gray, & Pepperberg, 2015;Parrish et al, 2014;Stevens, Rosati, Heilbronner, & Mühlhoff, 2011), food exchange tasks Dufour, Pelé, Sterck, & Thierry, 2007;Pelé, Dufour, Micheletta, & Thierry, 2010;Pelé, Micheletta, Uhlrich, Thierry, & Dufour, 2011;Ramseyer, Pelé, Dufour, Chauvin, & Thierry, 2006), token exchange tasks (e.g., Bourjade, Thierry, Call, & Dufour, 2012;Hackenberg & Vaidya, 2003;Judge & Essler, 2013;Parrish, Evans, Perdue, & Beran, 2013), and other tasks in which a more immediately available reward has to be avoide...…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We included two self-control tasks that are well known in the comparative literature and commonly used for assessing self-control in primates. The first task was the accumulation task in which food items were presented one at a time until the animal retrieved and consumed the items Beran, 2002;Beran & Evans, 2006;Evans & Beran, 2007;Parrish et al, 2014;Stevens et al, 2011;Vick, Bovet, & Anderson, 2010). The dependent measure of self-control was the number of items that an animal accumulated before consuming a food item.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%