2001
DOI: 10.1016/s0010-0277(01)00148-2
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Deep dyslexia in the two languages of an Arabic/French bilingual patient

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
53
0

Year Published

2006
2006
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
7
2
1

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 53 publications
(56 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
3
53
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Implementing the dissociation logic rests on the assumption that the patient's "qualitative deviation from intact behavior (the effect) originates in damage to a specific isolable component or components of the brain (the cause)" (Colangelo, Holden, Buchanan, & Van Orden, 2004, p. 151). Doubts about the assumption have been expressed in varied ways, from the lack of parsimony in explaining a bilingual deep dyslexic (Béland & Mimouni, 2001), to the absence of reliable modularity-defining criteria (e.g., Farah, 1994;Uttal, 2001), to the claim that qualitative changes are more likely to reflect self-organizing interaction-dominant dynamics, rather than a malfunctioning of autonomous components (e.g., Colangelo et al, 2004;Kello, 2003;Van Orden, Jansen op de Haar, & Bosman, 1997). Finally, the experimental method by which the impairments in single-case studies are revealed can also be questioned.…”
Section: Morton's (1969) Hypothesis Of a Single Lexicon Now Seems Thementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Implementing the dissociation logic rests on the assumption that the patient's "qualitative deviation from intact behavior (the effect) originates in damage to a specific isolable component or components of the brain (the cause)" (Colangelo, Holden, Buchanan, & Van Orden, 2004, p. 151). Doubts about the assumption have been expressed in varied ways, from the lack of parsimony in explaining a bilingual deep dyslexic (Béland & Mimouni, 2001), to the absence of reliable modularity-defining criteria (e.g., Farah, 1994;Uttal, 2001), to the claim that qualitative changes are more likely to reflect self-organizing interaction-dominant dynamics, rather than a malfunctioning of autonomous components (e.g., Colangelo et al, 2004;Kello, 2003;Van Orden, Jansen op de Haar, & Bosman, 1997). Finally, the experimental method by which the impairments in single-case studies are revealed can also be questioned.…”
Section: Morton's (1969) Hypothesis Of a Single Lexicon Now Seems Thementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Their review found that the error patterns of surface, phonological and deep dyslexia in monolingual speakers in different languages are observed in bilingual speakers [1,2,11,14,15,19,20,24,30,39]. That is not to say that all cases of acquired dyslexia in bilingual speakers show the same pattern of oral reading impairment in both languages.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Full assessments of ZT's language performance by Mimouni et al (1995) and Béland et al (2000) revealed that he displays 9 of the 12 features of the deep dyslexia syndrome in both languages (see Plaut and Shallice 1993). These features refer to the error pattern (semantic, visual, morphological errors) and to the type of stimuli that are more sensitive to reading errors (function words, verbs, abstract words, nonwords).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%