2007
DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2007.04.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Deconstructing rapid automatized naming: Component processes and the prediction of reading difficulties

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

3
42
0
2

Year Published

2011
2011
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8
2

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 49 publications
(47 citation statements)
references
References 73 publications
3
42
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…RAN tasks require participants to rapidly name a series of items presented in an array, and these tasks have been used extensively in studies of children's reading development (Denckla & Rudel, 1974, 1976see Swanson, Trainin, Necoechea, & Hammill, 2003, for a meta-analysis). Some studies have indicated that RAN performance reflects processing speed above and beyond reading ability (Cutting & Denckla, 2001;Powell, Stainthorp, Stuart, Garwood, & Quinlan, 2007;Kail & Hall, 1994;Kail, Hall, & Caskey, 1999;Savage, Pillay, & Melidona, 2007;Wolf & Bowers, 1999). Kuperman and Van Dyke (2011) demonstrated that RAN performance was one of the strongest unique predictors of eye-movement behavior during reading and suggested that the RAN task should be considered an index of oculomotor processing speed, which allows rapid coordination of lexical processing and eyemovement control (e.g., Reichle, Pollatsek, Fisher, & Rayner, 1998).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…RAN tasks require participants to rapidly name a series of items presented in an array, and these tasks have been used extensively in studies of children's reading development (Denckla & Rudel, 1974, 1976see Swanson, Trainin, Necoechea, & Hammill, 2003, for a meta-analysis). Some studies have indicated that RAN performance reflects processing speed above and beyond reading ability (Cutting & Denckla, 2001;Powell, Stainthorp, Stuart, Garwood, & Quinlan, 2007;Kail & Hall, 1994;Kail, Hall, & Caskey, 1999;Savage, Pillay, & Melidona, 2007;Wolf & Bowers, 1999). Kuperman and Van Dyke (2011) demonstrated that RAN performance was one of the strongest unique predictors of eye-movement behavior during reading and suggested that the RAN task should be considered an index of oculomotor processing speed, which allows rapid coordination of lexical processing and eyemovement control (e.g., Reichle, Pollatsek, Fisher, & Rayner, 1998).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There are several proponents of the first view, according to which RAN can be subsumed under phonological processing skills as it basically reflects an ability to efficiently retrieve phonological representations from long term memory, thus rendering it a useful measure of phonological access (Bowey et al., 2005;Kibby, Lee, & Dyer, 2014;Ramus 2014;Ramus & Szenkovits, 2008;Savage, Pillay, & Melidona, 2007;Torgesen, et al, 1997;Vaessen et al, 2009;Vellutino, Fletcher, Snowling, & Scanlon, 2004;Wagner et al, 1997). On a broader view of the construct of phonological skills, RAN has also been subsumed under this heading as a measure of lexical retrieval (i.e., access or retrieval of lexical phonological representations, Ramus & Szenkovits, 2008;Wagner & Torgesen, 1987).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The evidence related to naming speed deficits in individuals with dyslexia has been derived from correlational, longitudinal, and cross-sectional studies, as well as some reading-level studies (e.g., Ackerman & Dykman, 1993;Bowers, Steffy, & Tate, 1988;Bowers & Swanson, 1991;Chiappe, Stringer, Siegel, & Stanovich, 2002;Denckla & Rudel, 1976b;Kirby, Desrochers, Roth, & Lai, 2008;Korhonen, 1991;Korhonen, 1995;Manis, Doi, & Bhadha, 2000;Plaza & Cohen, 2003;Rudel, Denckla, & Broman, 1978;Savage, Pillay, & Melidona, 2007;Snyder & Downey, 1995;Wolf, 1982Wolf, , 1991. As indicated earlier only a brief overview of major findings is presented here.…”
Section: Cerebellar Deficit and Reading Disability 21mentioning
confidence: 99%