2016
DOI: 10.2139/ssrn.2734736
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Deception and Self-Deception

Abstract: Standard-Nutzungsbedingungen:Die Dokumente auf EconStor dürfen zu eigenen wissenschaftlichen Zwecken und zum Privatgebrauch gespeichert und kopiert werden.Sie dürfen die Dokumente nicht für öffentliche oder kommerzielle Zwecke vervielfältigen, öffentlich ausstellen, öffentlich zugänglich machen, vertreiben oder anderweitig nutzen.Sofern die Verfasser die Dokumente unter Open-Content-Lizenzen (insbesondere CC-Lizenzen) zur Verfügung gestellt haben sollten, gelten abweichend von diesen Nutzungsbedingungen die in… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
16
0
1

Year Published

2016
2016
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 28 publications
(20 citation statements)
references
References 72 publications
3
16
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…7 This implies that wrong updates are not purely noise, but seem to be partly driven by self-serving motives. The fraction of mistake updates in Table 1 is about the same as that found in MNNR, and four percentage points higher than in Schwardmann and Van der Weele (2016), studies that use the same belief elicitation mechanism and a similar feedback mechanism. 8 Table 1 demonstrates the advantages of eliciting feedback responsiveness in more than one task.…”
Section: Noise and Updating Mistakessupporting
confidence: 60%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…7 This implies that wrong updates are not purely noise, but seem to be partly driven by self-serving motives. The fraction of mistake updates in Table 1 is about the same as that found in MNNR, and four percentage points higher than in Schwardmann and Van der Weele (2016), studies that use the same belief elicitation mechanism and a similar feedback mechanism. 8 Table 1 demonstrates the advantages of eliciting feedback responsiveness in more than one task.…”
Section: Noise and Updating Mistakessupporting
confidence: 60%
“…However, it is not clear how robust these findings are. Schwardmann and Van der Weele (2016) and Coutts (2016) provide replications of the MNNR framework. While both papers find substantial conservatism in updating, they do not replicate the asymmetry result of MNNR, and even find a tendency to overweight negative rather than positive signals.…”
Section: Literaturementioning
confidence: 99%
“…A manipulação das respostas (ou dos comportamentos) pode ocorrer de forma pensada, em que a pessoa sabe que a sua resposta (ou comportamento) não condiz com o seu traço latente. Ainda, essa manipulação pode ocorrer de forma não consciente, de modo que a pessoa responde de forma socialmente desejável por acreditar que possui tais características positivas, ignorando os seus traços menos desejáveis (Schwardmann & van der Weele, 2016).…”
Section: Escala De Gerenciamento Da Impressão E Autoengano -Ipip: Um unclassified
“…Köszegi (2006), for example, focuses on the role of “ego utility” (utility from believing oneself able to complete a challenging task) in generating overconfidence. Bénabou and Tirole (2002) propose, in addition to this consumption motive, two additional explanations for overconfidence: a signalling motive (greater self‐belief makes it easier to convince others of one's competence: see also Schwardmann and Van der Weele 2019) and a motivational benefit whereby believing oneself to be competent might increase persistence towards goals (Fischer and Sliwka 2018).…”
Section: Literaturementioning
confidence: 99%