2011
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.09.007
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Counterfactual thinking and false belief: The role of executive function

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

1
30
0
3

Year Published

2012
2012
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 84 publications
(35 citation statements)
references
References 46 publications
1
30
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…This might put a greater burden on their working memory, causing an overload, which results in falling back on BCR and consequently a wrong answer to the test question. Drayton and colleagues (2011) reported that 5-year-old children who performed better on counterfactual tasks had higher working memory scores. In their study, children received antecedent and consequent counterfactual tasks.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This might put a greater burden on their working memory, causing an overload, which results in falling back on BCR and consequently a wrong answer to the test question. Drayton and colleagues (2011) reported that 5-year-old children who performed better on counterfactual tasks had higher working memory scores. In their study, children received antecedent and consequent counterfactual tasks.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A clear sign of children being able to do so is their passing the false belief test. Several studies showed that CFR abilities are related to explicit understanding of false belief (Ecker, 2011; German & Nichols, 2003; Grant, Riggs, & Boucher, 2004; Guajardo & Turley-Ames, 2004; Hofer, 2010; Müller, Miller, Michalczyk, & Karapinka, 2007; Perner, Sprung, & Steinkogler, 2004; Riggs et al, 1998) and that inhibitory control is a partially mediating factor (Drayton, Turley-Ames, & Guajardo, 2011). Inhibition has also been found to predict performance on counterfactual tasks (Beck, Riggs, & Gorniak, 2009), but not whether they experience counterfactual emotions such as regret (Burns et al, 2012).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…mental representations of alternatives to past events [36] that, in this case, involve both the self and the other. Thinking about hypothetical events has been linked to executive functioning, especially working memory and inhibitory control [37]. Apart from engaging additional cognitive competencies, this level is likely to also pose more demands on mentalizing skills as chimpanzees and children do not show context effects in a modified UG [11], [12], [38].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, we added three 'neutral' words to the final target sentence of each item (identical across conditions within an item). Secondly, previous research has begun to demonstrate the importance of working memory in counterfactual thinking, with studies showing that counterfactual judgements are impaired under high working memory load (Goldinger, Kleider, Azuma, & Beike, 2003), and that reduced working memory capacity correlates with poorer performance on tasks that require counterfactual thinking (Drayton, Turley-Ames, & Guajardo, 2011). However, we are aware of no study that has examined how working memory might influence the incremental processing of counterfactual utterances compared to factual utterances (that only activate representation of a single version of the world).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%