2015
DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2015.05.024
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Examining the cognitive costs of counterfactual language comprehension: Evidence from ERPs

Abstract: Recent empirical research suggests that understanding a counterfactual event (e.g. 'If Josie had revised, she would have passed her exams') activates mental representations of both the factual and counterfactual versions of events. However, it remains unclear when readers switch between these models during comprehension, and whether representing multiple 'worlds' is cognitively effortful. This paper reports two ERP studies where participants read contexts that set up a factual or counterfactual scenario, follo… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
45
2

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 22 publications
(53 citation statements)
references
References 53 publications
(55 reference statements)
6
45
2
Order By: Relevance
“…This pattern can be seen as supporting the notion that the interpretation of counterfactuals is strict, as assumed by Lewis (1973), and that most counterfactuals are false Hájek (2014): It is difficult to prove and easy to disprove them, especially when they are based on outlandish premises. We have also found evidence that implicates working memory in counterfactual processing, which is expected if mental simulation of possible worlds is involved, and matches previous evidence from the processing literature (Ferguson and Cane, 2015). Based on the finding that high-capacity participants are more likely to give FALSE judgments for counterfactuals, and especially in implausible cases, our preliminary conclusion is that these individuals may strategically allocate mental resources to falsification, which increases their efficiency at performing the task.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 86%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This pattern can be seen as supporting the notion that the interpretation of counterfactuals is strict, as assumed by Lewis (1973), and that most counterfactuals are false Hájek (2014): It is difficult to prove and easy to disprove them, especially when they are based on outlandish premises. We have also found evidence that implicates working memory in counterfactual processing, which is expected if mental simulation of possible worlds is involved, and matches previous evidence from the processing literature (Ferguson and Cane, 2015). Based on the finding that high-capacity participants are more likely to give FALSE judgments for counterfactuals, and especially in implausible cases, our preliminary conclusion is that these individuals may strategically allocate mental resources to falsification, which increases their efficiency at performing the task.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 86%
“…Readers with higher working memory capacity may engage with counterfactual statements more deeply, considering more possibilities, than readers with lower working memory capacity. Furthermore, given that mental simulation taxes working memory (Ferguson and Cane, 2015;Van Hoeck et al, 2015), high-capacity readers may be able to represent a higher number of possible worlds within their modal horizon compared to low-capacity readers. Assuming that deviation from the ostensible truth value of a given counterfactual is a signal that the modal horizon has been expanded, high-capacity readers should thus show more deviations than low-capacity readers.…”
Section: Research Questions and Predictionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Subjects' sensitivity to contextual constraints and other pragmatic abilities (e.g. Ferguson et al 2014;Nieuwland et al 2010) as well as working memory capacity (Ferguson and Cane 2015) are possible candidates.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Our results show that within the short counterfactual sentences used here, readers favoured the counterfactual world for processing incoming information online. However, given the large body of empirical evidence that has demonstrated dual representations for counterfactuals (Ferguson, 2012;Ferguson & Cane, 2015de Vega et al, 2007;Gomez-Veiga et al, 2010, Santamaria et al, 2005Urrutia et al, 2012), it is expected that this initial counterfactual interpretation would be weakened if a longer delay was introduced between the counterfactual antecedent and the target word, resulting in reduced or delayed anomaly/implausibility detection effects. Further research is necessary to explore this.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…That is, reading times on the critical word (first-pass fixation duration and total reading time) did not differ between counterfactual-consistent (ÒcarrotsÓ) and -inconsistent (ÒfishÓ) continuations. Further evidence for dual-representations activated by counterfactuals can be seen in Ferguson (2012), Ferguson and Cane (2015), de Vega, Urrutia, and Riffo (2007), de Vega & Urrutia, (2012), Gomez-Veiga, Garcia-Madruga, and Moreno-Rios (2010), Santamaria, Espino, & Byrne (2005), and Urrutia, de Vega, and Bastiaansen (2012).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 95%