1993
DOI: 10.2190/9l88-tjvj-xhg9-ut12
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Controlling Challenge in Instructional Simulation

Abstract: This experiment was designed to assess the effects of instructional control mechanisms that altered the degree of challenge in an instructional simulation. Subjects were taught to place an icon at a position and orientation that corresponded to digital and analogue information displayed on a simulated aviation instrument. Three treatments were compared: a) learner control enabled subjects to control challenge; b) learner control with advisement provided the same control accompanied by ongoing advice on control… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

0
6
0

Year Published

1994
1994
2004
2004

Publication Types

Select...
3
1
1

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 16 publications
0
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Typically, learner-controlled training tasks last from 30 to 60 minutes (e.g., Burwell, 1991;Ellermann & Free, 1990;Gray, 1989;Lai, 2001;Mattoon & Klein, 1993;Milheim, 1990;Ross & Rakow, 1981). Cronbach and Snow (1981), however, note, "With most of the instructional variables examined in ATI [aptitude-treatment interaction] research, a period of habituation is probably necessary before the student is working with full effectiveness; this leads us to believe that an experiment lasting any less than ten class periods will be uninformative" (p. 44).…”
Section: Guideline # 2: Give It Timementioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Typically, learner-controlled training tasks last from 30 to 60 minutes (e.g., Burwell, 1991;Ellermann & Free, 1990;Gray, 1989;Lai, 2001;Mattoon & Klein, 1993;Milheim, 1990;Ross & Rakow, 1981). Cronbach and Snow (1981), however, note, "With most of the instructional variables examined in ATI [aptitude-treatment interaction] research, a period of habituation is probably necessary before the student is working with full effectiveness; this leads us to believe that an experiment lasting any less than ten class periods will be uninformative" (p. 44).…”
Section: Guideline # 2: Give It Timementioning
confidence: 99%
“…In other words, learners are given command over instructional options that were traditionally instructor-or programcontrolled (Ross & Rakow, 1981). Since Mager coined the term in 1961, learner control has grown to include control of many instructional design elements, including control of the content, sequence, pacing, context within which to learn, method of presentation, provision of optional content, locus of instructional control, incentives, and task difficulty of instruction (Mattoon & Klein, 1993;Ross & Rakow, 1981;Sims & Hedberg, 1995). In classroom training, instructors generally direct learning tasks, and training is often tailored to meet the needs of the average trainee.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This assumption is contrary to the findings of educational research and may lead to the development of poor training simulations (Andrews, 1988). Mattoon and Klein (1993) point out that the additional tasks leamers must engage in to control an instructional simu• may produce performance deficits. They administered a whole-task -,%i lesson that taught undergraduates how to estimate the location and heading of target symbols on a simulated radar display.…”
Section: Instructional Controlmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Yet, most empirical evidence is in favor of program control. Instructional control strategies have not been thoroughly examined in the context of instructional simulation, but the findings of two studies (Mattoon & Klein, 1993;Munro, Fehling, and Town, 1985) indicate that instructional support must be carefully controlled to avoid interfering with learners' concentration on complex practice tasks.…”
Section: Instructional Controlmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation