2011
DOI: 10.1007/s10071-010-0377-y
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Contrafreeloading and the value of control over visual stimuli in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata)

Abstract: Contrafreeloading, which means that animals work for food even though identical food is freely available, has been reported in animals' feeding behavior. This phenomenon has been assumed to be explained by the information primacy model, in which the information about a food resource as well as the food itself is valuable for animals. This study confirmed a contrafreeloading-like phenomenon using movies as rewards rather than food in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata) and investigated the motivational system th… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
23
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 27 publications
(28 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
0
23
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In addition, increasing quantity and numerosity may intensify reward (Parrish, Evans, & Beran, 2015;Petrazzini & Wynne, 2016). Moreover, actively dealing with a situation or exerting choice may increase a situation's attraction (Niv, Langdon, & Radulescu, 2015; e.g., contra-freeloading: Inglis, Forkman, & Lazarus, 1997;Ogura, 2011). Scarcity of a desired resource (Sehnert, Franks, Yap, & Higgins, 2014), the challenge of a task or the effort needed to reach a goal (Kurzban, Duckworth, Kable, & Myers, 2013;Nishiyama, 2016;Shadmehr, Huang, & Ahmed, 2016) may be rewarding at low but not at high intensity.…”
Section: Valuating a Set Of Stimuli-reactive And Proactive Emotionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition, increasing quantity and numerosity may intensify reward (Parrish, Evans, & Beran, 2015;Petrazzini & Wynne, 2016). Moreover, actively dealing with a situation or exerting choice may increase a situation's attraction (Niv, Langdon, & Radulescu, 2015; e.g., contra-freeloading: Inglis, Forkman, & Lazarus, 1997;Ogura, 2011). Scarcity of a desired resource (Sehnert, Franks, Yap, & Higgins, 2014), the challenge of a task or the effort needed to reach a goal (Kurzban, Duckworth, Kable, & Myers, 2013;Nishiyama, 2016;Shadmehr, Huang, & Ahmed, 2016) may be rewarding at low but not at high intensity.…”
Section: Valuating a Set Of Stimuli-reactive And Proactive Emotionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Their instincts, behaviour and needs, however, are likely to be the same as those of their wild conspecifics, since most parrots are only one or two generations removed from the wild and thus not considered as domesticated species (Davis, 1998;Graham, 1998). Similar to other animal species such as rats, pigs, monkeys, starlings, pigeons and chickens (Inglis et al, 1997;Lindqvist et al, 2002;de Leeuw, 2004;Ogura, 2011), parrots will contrafreeload (i.e., work for food even when food is freely available), suggesting that there is a behavioural need to forage (Coulton et al, 1997;van Zeeland et al, 2009;Joseph, 2010). This behavioural need, which is controlled by both physiological and psychological parameters, can only be satiated if an animal performs and experiences the consequences of both the appetitive and consummatory phases of feeding (Collier and RovĂ©e-Collier, 1981;Hughes and Duncan, 1988;Jensen and Toates, 1993).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Another phenomenon is contrafreeloading, which experimentally evaluates the value of control as well as reward which can be obtained as a result of control (Inglis et al, 1997;Sambrook and Buchanan-Smith, 1997). This phenomenon was confirmed by using a Video system enrichment for Japanese macaques 7 visual reward (Ogura, 2011) as well as a food reward (Menzel, 1991;Reinhardt, 1994) in primates. These studies provided empirical evidence of the value of control over the environment for primates.…”
Section: Controllability Over Videosmentioning
confidence: 63%